|
|
Dogs’ special domestication processes, their natural socialization to humans, and the possibility of tracing evolutionary changes by comparing dogs’ behavior to that of wolves, make dogs altogether unique for studying the evolution of complex social behavior. Here the authors report some much needed comparisons between the behavior of dogs and wolves. The authors reveal some dog-specific behaviors, especially with regard to their interactions with humans, by comparing dogs and wolves hand-reared identically. This approach ensures that behavioral differences between dogs and wolves will be due to species-specific (genetic) differences, and not to differences in experience. The results indicate that social attraction, presumably synchronizing behavior, and communicative abilities of dogs changed markedly during the process of domestication. The authors suggest that this model of dog behavior has the potential to provide new insights into the evolution of human socio-cognitive behavior. |
|
Introduction
One of the most influential books on the experimental
study of social behavior development was based on a 15-year-long study
of dogs. Scott and Fuller (1965) argued that the study of dogs provides
an understanding of developmental and genetic factors in social
behavior, but additionally, because dogs evolved for living in a human
dominated niche, the study of dogs has a special reference to the
development of human social behavior. Accordingly dog puppies –
similarly to neonates of other mammalian species – provide a good
homologous model for the development of social behavior in human
children. However, mainly because of the behavioral genetics approach,
the experimental fell short of providing a clear ethological support for
these ideas. The results had restricted relevance to those behavioral
features that we today regard as parts of social cognition. The
naturalistic observation of communicative and cooperative behavior
between humans and dogs was lacking; humans functioned only as social
reward. Finally, the dogs were maintained in conspecific family groups,
mainly in kennels, and had only limited contact with humans. Thus,
without denying the important contribution of these early studies to the
understanding of socio-cognitive behavior in the dog, a more
ethologically oriented approach is clearly needed.
Dogs have three basic features, which make the
species altogether unique for studying the evolution of complex social
behavior. First, during evolution the behavior of dogs changed in a way
that made them successful in the human social environment. Second, the
behavior of dogs’ ancestor species can be reconstructed from the
behavior of the wolf. Therefore, one can trace the changes that occurred
during domestication that led to the emergence of a unique companion
species for humans. Third, the natural socialization of dogs in the
human environment offers a parallel between them and human children.
Thus we suggest that the detailed ethological study of dog behavior
could provide a functionally analogous model to the early evolutionary
stages of human socio-cognitive behavior (Miklósi, Topál, & Csányi,
2004; Hare & Tomasello, 2005).
This naturalistic model considers the dog a member of
a human social group, that is, the social niche of family dogs is the
human social environment. Here we report ethologically inspired
behavioral observations and experimental work carried out in the
mixed-species group of dogs and humans.
Unique evolutionary history in the human niche
Various populations of Homo sapiens sapiens
left Africa in a range of 50,000-100,000 years ago (Finlayson, 2005) and
moved to Southeast Asia and Europe. These were the first people to meet
wolves. Based on evolutionary genetic arguments, this process may have
started as early as 40,000 years ago (Savolainen, Zhang, Ling,
Lundeberg, & Leitner, 2002), but archeological evidence exists only for
much later events (12,000-15,000 years ago; Davis & Valla, 1978; Nobis,
1979; see Figure 1). However, this discrepancy can be explained partly
by assuming that genetic - and probably behavioral - separation of the
to-be-dog and wolf populations preceded the emergence of any detectable
anatomical change (Vilá et al., 1997). In any case, dogs were the first
domesticated animals.
Figure 1. Phylogenetic tree
showing the evolutionary interrelationships among
species (lines) and their ecological niche (colored
areas). Approximately 40,000 years ago, dogs entered the
human social environment. |
|
Some researchers assume that domestication started
with a population of wolves that became able to exploit food resources
provided by humans (Coppinger & Coppinger, 2001). Later humans
encouraged these wolves to join them. Subsequent selection for different
behaviors and certain preferred appearances concluded with the emergence
of dog breeds. It is interesting to note that dogs usually found their
way to join human groups despite the variability in human social systems
and cultural traditions around the world.
Additionally, it is remarkable that dogs developed
close contact with humans some 3,000-5,000 years earlier than any other
species. As far as we know, dogs were not domesticated for any direct
benefit (e.g., food). As early dog fossils from burials indicate, dogs
had a special, probably partly spiritual, relationship with humans from
very early on (Morey, 2006).
Behavior of the ancestor
Genetic research identified the wolf as the nearest
evolutionary relative of the dog (Vilá et al., 1997). This fact provides
a very effective comparative background, even though wolves have been
distributed across the entire Holarctic and are represented by
genetically and behaviorally different populations. Additionally, wolves
have suffered from human hunting and environmental destruction in most
parts of the world, which probably changed their behavior in many
respects (e.g., increased their homophobia).
Field research on wolves (e.g., Mech, 1970; Mech &
Boitani, 2003) has revealed that the complexity of wolf social behavior
can be compared to that observed in primates. Wolves live in closed
family groups. Their dominance relationships are context-dependent (Fox,
1972); kinship relations, alliances and friendships complicate the
picture (Fox, 1973). Although the general rule is that the alpha pair
has an exclusive right for mating, individual preferences often override
this habit (Rabb, Woolpy, & Ginsburg, 1967). Members of the pack perform
cooperative behavior in hunting and when raising the offspring (Altman,
1987). Wolves possess very expressive means of communication in various
visual, auditory, and chemical signals (Schenkel, 1967).
All in all, the evolution of the wolf resulted in a
set of complex social skills which probably contributed to the success
of this species. The presence of these behavioral features provided a
fortunate situation in which, by changing some aspects of their social
behavior, wolves were able to adapt to the human social niche during an
early phase of the modern Homo’s evolution. Given that environmental
factors and experiences of the individual are comparable (see below),
the differences in socio-cognitive behavior between dogs and wolves
should point to those behavioral aspects that were affected during the
domestication process.
Naturalistic socialization with humans
Socialization in the human environment can be
regarded as a natural process in the dog. This does not exclude the
possibility that dogs can survive and establish conspecific groups in
the absence of human contact. Although we should point out that, if
early human contact is provided, many individuals of even a wild species
can be socialized to humans, dogs’ long evolutionary history places them
in a unique position. In contrast to wolves, whose socialization to
humans has to begin before day 10 (eye opening) (Klinghammer & Goodmann,
1987; Frank, Frank, Hasselbach, & Littleton, 1989), the time window for
socialization in dogs (Scott & Fuller, 1965), probably closes at around
12 weeks (Freedman, King, & Elliot, 1961). Social attraction to humans
develops even after 20-minute encounters twice a week, or daily eye
contact with humans (Scott & Fuller, 1965). Moreover, approach
tendencies toward the human cannot be diminished by punishment (electric
shocks) of the puppies (Fox & Stelzner 1966) (see also below).
The socialization level of dogs varies among
individuals (just as is the case with children), but exposure to the
human social environment is a natural consequence of their evolutionary
history and not merely an experimental or procedural variable. In
addition, dogs’ social environments and experiences in many respects
correspond to that of children. Some data suggest that there is a close
similarity between how humans interact with young infants and with dogs
(Mitchell, 2001). If questioned, dog owners regard their dog (although
with marked variation) not only as a member of the family (Cain, 1983)
but they also felt, their relationship with their dog was similar to the
relationship they maintained with their own child (Berryman, Howells, &
Lloyd-Evans, 1985). Additionally, 6.5% of randomly selected veterinary
center clients said they could imagine certain circumstances in which
they would give a scarce drug to their pet dog in preference to a person
outside the family (Cohen, 2002). Therefore, the comparison of human
infants and dogs raise the possibility to investigate how two organisms
with very different evolutionary paths behave after having been exposed
to a similar social environment (Gomez, 2005).
Family Wolf Project: Comparison of human-oriented
behavior in intensively socialized wolves and dogs
To date, only a few systematic comparative research
programs were carried out (Frank, 1987; Zimen, 1987; Fox, 1971), but
even these endeavors differed in many respects in their focus and
initial working hypotheses. Further, wolf research has suffered from
many methodological problems. Wolves are notoriously difficult animals
to observe in the nature because their lifestyle is extremely dynamic,
and they prefer remote territories (Mech, 1970). Captive animals were
often forced to live under unnatural physical and social circumstances
and thus provide only a limited insight into the life of this species
(see Packard, 2003). Therefore, it is not surprising that there has been
much disagreement on the structure and function of wolf packs and on
many behavioral aspects of wolf life (for a recent review see Mech &
Boitani, 2003).
From our comparative experimental perspective,
further problems emerge by the various socialization procedures applied
in captive wolf research. Earlier we have argued (Miklósi, Topál, &
Csányi, 2004) that in order to obtain comparative experimental results,
the physical and social experiences of the two species have to be at a
comparable level. For this purpose two approaches seem to be obvious. As
for wild wolves, we should compare them with free-ranging dogs. However,
in contrast to wolves, free-ranging dog packs usually have a continuous
influx from dogs that previously lived in the vicinity of humans
(Boitani, Francisci, & Ciucci, 1995). Additionally, we expect that
domestication affected the relationship with humans, primarily, and less
affected the intraspecies social behavior. Therefore, the only
reasonable possibility to reconstruct the domestication process from
comparative data is to expose wolves and dogs to similar amounts of
experience with humans. This approach ensures that the behavioral
differences between dogs and wolves will be due to species-specific
(genetic) differences, and not due to the lack of experiences (e.g.,
neophobia) or destructive effect of the experimental procedures. As we
suggested above, humans can only socialize wolves successfully if cubs
are separated from their mothers before eye opening and spend at least
20 hours in close human contact (Klinghammer & Goodman 1987; Frank et
al., 1989); we decided follow this socialization regime with both with
wolves and dogs.
The socialization procedure
Earlier studies on socialized wolves’ behavior tested
problem solving, learning ability, or intraspecies aggression and did
not report on human oriented behavior. In addition, the sample sizes in
this work were often low (Fentress, 1967; Frank & Frank, 1982), and
wolves and dogs were not socialized to comparable levels (Hare, Brown,
Williamson, & Tomasello, 2002).
We decided to avoid these insufficiencies. In the
years 2001-2003 we obtained 13, 4 to 6 day-old wolf cubs from 5 litters
(6 males and 7 females in all) and 11 dog puppies of similar ages from 5
litters (6 males and 5 females; all mongrels, born in shelters). The
unique feature of this program was that each cub and puppy had its own
human caretaker, who spent 24 hours a day together with the animal for a
period of about 9-16 weeks (Figure 2). Although the animals had the
chance to meet conspecifics regularly (at least weekly), they spent most
of their time in close contact with the human caretaker. Additionally,
every caretaker took special care to avoid competitive situations and
aggressive interactions in order to maintain a friendly and trustful
relationship with the animal.
The caretakers often carried the animals on their
body in a pouch, and they slept together at night. The animals were fed
first by bottle, then later (from the age of 4-5 weeks) with solid food
by hand. When the subjects’ motor activity made it possible, they were
trained to walk on leash and execute
some basic obedience tasks. The
caretakers carried the cubs and the puppies to various places either by
car or public transport. Also, they were regular visitors at the
university, participated in dog-camps, and frequented dog training
schools (Figure 3).
From their 3rd week of life the animals were tested
regularly (from 3 to 9 weeks, weekly, except at the 7th week) in a
laboratory at the university. These behavioral experiments tested for
social preferences, social and physical neophobia, reaction to
dominance, retrieval of objects, communication with humans, and possessivity.
After this intensive period, the wolves were
gradually integrated into a wolf pack at Gödöll (near Budapest), and the
caretakers visited them once or twice a week. These visits included
social contact and joint exercises (going for a walk), training of
obedience tasks (sitting and lying down for food reward), and
experimental work, which was terminated when the wolves became 1.5-2
years old. The dogs received the same intensive socialization procedure
as the wolves, but they either stayed with their caretaker, or we found
adoptive caretakers for them.
Figure 3. This is what
intensive socialization means! Wolves visited dog
schools and cities and participated in Christmas
parties and TV shows. |
|
Tractability of our intensively socialized wolves
Due to the intensive socialization, subsequent
training, and testing, it was easy for the caretakers to handle the
wolves even after the juvenile period. To demonstrate this, we examined
the behavior of 8 wolves (1 male and 2 females, aged 2 years; 4 females
and 1 male, aged 1 year) in a set of tractability tasks (Ujfalussy,
Virányi, & Kubinyi, 2003). The other 5 intensively socialized animals
were not available at the time of testing; however, they were kept and
treated in the same way as their mates. The animals were observed only
once in the test battery presented below, therefore the data are only
for giving an indication.
Response
to calling by name in the pack. The
animals were kept together as a pack, with other (not intensively
socialized, 3- or 4-year-old) individuals in a 10,000 m2 enclosure. The
caretaker stepped in the enclosure, shouted her animal’s name several
times, and tried to touch the approaching wolf. All animals responded to
the call and approached the caretaker in less than 46 seconds (latency
of the first touch from the first call: mean (sec) ± SE = 22.3 ± 5.3).
The caretaker held the wolf, put a collar and leash on it, and the
caretaker and wolf left the enclosure. During the subsequent tasks, the
tested animal was separated from its mates in a kennel, where the
caretaker and a cameraman that the animal was familiar with were
present.
Sitting
and lying down. The caretaker instructed
the wolf to sit and to lie down twice. The caretaker had a food pellet
in her hand, held her arm above the wolf, and said "Sit!" in Hungarian.
When the wolf sat down, the caretaker crouched, and said "Lie down!" in
Hungarian. Every wolf performed the tasks. Then a woman (aged 23) with
whom the wolves were not familiar repeated the same actions. Every wolf
without exception performed the tasks again.
The sitting down task was repeated by both persons.
Now the aim was to keep the wolf sitting as long as possible. The length
of time spent sitting during the 2 trials was averaged (caretaker: mean
(sec) ± SE = 10.9 ± 2.2; unfamiliar woman: mean (sec) ± SE = 8.7 ± 2.1).
There was no difference between obeying the two persons (t 5
= 2.1, p = 0.09).
Wearing
dog accessories. The wolves were
accustomed to wearing muzzles; therefore, it was not surprising that
every animal let the caretaker put a muzzle on them, without any sign of
disagreement, in less than 15 seconds (mean (sec) ± SE = 10.5 ± 1.6).
Similarly, all of the wolves allowed the caretaker to put dog socks on
one of their front and one of their hind paws, although they had never
had such an experience before (mean (sec) ± SE = 101.8 ± 34.3). Only 2
animals pulled off the socks in the subsequent 30 seconds.
However, when the unfamiliar woman tried to put a
collar on the wolves, 4 animals (from both age-groups) showed aggressive
signs, so we stopped the test. The other 4 animals accepted the
well-known collar from the person they were not familiar with (mean
(sec) ± SE = 33.5 ± 6.8).
Social
and physical neophobia. The unfamiliar
woman invited the wolf to play with a toy. Each animal approached her, 2
played with the toy, 2 were neutral, and 4 (all females) showed the sign
of mild stress (held their tail between their legs). In another
neophobia test, we observed how our animals responded to a 1 m high flag
barrier (10 cm wide red strips are attached to a string with a 10 cm
separation between them). This kind of barrier was commonly used to hunt
or live-trap animals in Eastern Europe. The general experience is that
unsocialized wolves are afraid of passing through such a barrier
(Musiani et al., 2003). However, 6 out of our 7 wolves could be enticed
to pass through this flag barrier within a minute. A 2-year-old female
crossed it without any kind of encouragement.
Overall, our wolves could be handled similarly to
dogs in many ways, with moderate individual differences (Ujfalussy et
al., 2003). Therefore, intensive early handling proved to be an
effective means of socializing wolves to a level comparable to dogs,
which made comparative behavioral experiments possible. Previous
attempts at intensive socialization of wolves were usually restricted to
a single animal (e.g., Fentress, 1967), which made comparative work
questionable. Additionally, some comparative work involved either less
socialized wolves than the dogs that were used for comparisons (e.g.,
Hare, Brown, Williamson, & Tomasello, 2002) or deliberately utilized a
mixed socialization procedure. For example, the wolves reared by Frank &
Frank (1982) spent 12 hours with their mother and littermates and 12
hours with the experimenters and 1 littermate between the 11th day and
6th week. After following this protocol, Frank et al. (1989) reported
that their wolf cubs were nearly as wary of humans as were cubs reared
without human contact. Yet, the most frequently used protocol is to take
away every cub from the mother before the 10th day, keep the cubs
together, and bottle-feed them in the first few weeks. After two months
the wolves usually live in a kennel as a pack (Feddersen-Pettersen,
1991; Hare, Brown, Williamson, & Tomasello, 2002). In contrast, we
studied a fairly large sample of wolves that were separated both from
their mother and their littermates on days 4-6 and stayed with humans
for up to 3-4 months. For comparative purposes, we also studied a group
of intensively socialized dogs. In our view, this represents an
important first step in comparing the social behavior toward humans of
wolves and dogs.
Dog Behavior From a Comparative Perspective
Early comparative projects had various research
agendas, which often led to paradoxical results. For example, when the
famous German zoologist, Bernard Grzimek (1942), proposed that
domestication might have enhanced the dog’s cognitive abilities, the
reason behind this was probably – at least partly – due to his bad
experiences in training socialized wolves. In subsequent experiments, he
found that his dogs performed better at recalling the location of cached
food than wolves did. In contrast, Hemmer (1990) assumed that the
domestication process had a detrimental effect on cognitive abilities of
dogs (and other species) because the environment provided by humans
buffered these species against selective forces that have an effect on
natural populations. Frank (1980) hypothesized that the main difference
between wolf and dog lies in the way they process information. According
to him, wolves possess an instinctual system, which is responsible for
behavioral traits needed for survival, while the cognitive system
controls complex behavioral skills needed for social life and hunting.
Evolution merged these two systems in dogs, which paved the way for a
more flexible behavioral organization that reacts to a much wider range
of stimuli and shows a more flexible behavioral adjustment in comparison
to their ancestor.
We would not deny that there are many interesting
ideas in these approaches, but given the very limited comparative
experimental evidence, a definite theory on the underlying behavioral
and cognitive machinery is premature. As a working hypothesis, we assume
that dogs’ socio-cognitive abilities were shaped by human social setting
(see above); therefore, we categorize the behavior elements under
investigation in accordance with the scheme of Human Behavior Complex
(HBC) developed by Csányi (2000). HBC has three main dimensions:
sociality, behavioral synchronization, and constructivity. One advantage
of this approach is that it decomposes complex social behavior into
smaller traits which can be investigated separately in a comparative
perspective. The value of the comparative model will depend on to what
degree the counterparts of the HBC complex can be experimentally tested
and evaluated in different species. Based on the HBC we have introduced
the Dog Behavior Complex (DBC; Topál, Miklósi, et al., 2006). Here we
cover those elements of DBC that have a particular relevance from a
wolf-dog comparison perspective.
Figure 4. Mean Preference Indexes
of 5-week-old dogs and wolves (+SE). The index was
calculated as: (relative duration of time spent with
caregiver – relative duration of time spent with other
stimulus)/(relative duration of time spent with
caregiver + relative duration of time spent with other
stimulus). *indicates significant difference (*p<.05,
**p<.01). Comparing the preference index to zero, dogs
spent more time with caregiver than with the adult dog,
but preference vanished when the experimenter played the
role of the competing social stimulus. In the case of
wolves the results were the opposite (p<0.01). From
"Species-Specific Differences and Similarities in the
Behavior of Hand Raised Dog and Wolf Puppies in Social
Situations with Humans," by M. Gácsi, B. Győri, Á. Miklósi, Zs. Virányi, E. Kubinyi, J. Topál, and V.
Csányi, 2005, Developmental Psychobiology, 47, p. 117.
Copyright 2005 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Reprinted with
permission of the author. |
|
Figure 5. Relative frequency of
aggressive behavior when the familiar experimenter tried
to put down the subjects at the beginning and pick them
up at the end of tests. Boxes indicate the 50% of the
data (lower and upper interquartile range). Whiskers
extend to the smallest and largest values excluding
outliers and extremities. None of the dog puppies
behaved aggressively in contrast to the wolf cubs.
*indicates significant differences (*p<0.05; **<0.01).
|
|
Socialization and social attraction
Early
social preference. Wolf cubs socialized by
humans show species specific affective behaviors toward their
caregivers, but whether this reflects a change in preference (relative
to conspecifics) was not tested. Frank and Frank (1982) found that
wolves exposed both to conspecifics and humans showed a preference for
the canid partner if they were offered a choice. This was in contrast to
Malamute pups, which displayed more signs of preference for humans, and
although they maintained their relationship with their mother, the dogs
rapidly developed strong emotional ties with humans. However, recently,
we have shown that intensively socialized wolf cubs did not prefer to
stay in the proximity of a conspecific at the age of 3-5 weeks if their
caretaker was also present (Gácsi et al., 2005; Figure 4). This result
indicates that more intensive socialization than that of Frank and Frank
(1982) can inhibit preference toward conspecifics, at least at this
early age. By way of comparison, hand-reared dogs were more vocal,
approached humans more frequently, wagged their tails, and looked at the
human’s face, which was not observed in wolves (Gácsi et al., 2005).
These interesting results do not answer all questions
about dogs’ social preferences. We know from previous experiments that
dogs easily develop preferences for other species, such as rabbits
(Cairns & Werboff, 1967) and cats (Fox, 1970), if raised together with
these species early in life. Therefore, it is not sure whether dogs’
preference for humans is the result of a special selection in the human
environment, or whether dogs have a decreased specificity for accepting
heterospecifics as group mates. Also, dogs and wolves are not the only
species that can be disposed toward accepting humans as social partners.
Selection for tameness affects the socialization periods and the
predisposition towards humans in silver foxes (Belyaev, 1978; Plyusnina,
Oskina, & Trut, 1991).
Aggressive behavior. Wolves are often
characterized as aggressive animals, based upon the behavior in captive
packs, but experienced field observers describe them usually as
relatively peaceful, and increased aggression is restricted mainly to
certain periods of social life (e.g., mating season; Mech, 1999;
Packard, 2003). Given the wide range of aggressive behavior in dogs, the
hypothesized overall decrement of aggressive behavior in dogs is
difficult to support experimentally. Interestingly, long-term
observation of wolves and dogs (Poodles) during their first year of life
proved that juvenile wolves were less aggressive (Feddersen-Pettersen,
1991). Similarly, Frank and Frank (1982) comparing wolves and Malamutes
could not find enhanced aggressiveness in cubs of the former species.
We conducted several behavioral experiments weekly in
a laboratory between the first 3-9 weeks of the animals’ lives. Tests
usually began when the animal was placed on a starting point by a
familiar experimenter. After the test the experimenter picked up the
subject and handed it to the caregiver. None of the dog puppies behaved
aggressively in the 131 interactions with the familiar experimenter
during the tests, but among the 13 wolves there were 9 individuals that
growled at and/or tried to bite the experimenter (in 41 cases out of 143
interactions or 29%). The greater number of growls and attacks (e.g.,
attempted biting) in wolves and the absence of these behaviors in the
dogs could be best explained by supposing that wolves either did not
like to be touched or constrained in their movements, or they had a
lower threshold for the elicitation of aggressive behavior (Gyri, 2004;
see also Gácsi et al., 2005; Figure 5).
We cannot report on aggressive behavior of our wolves
at an adult age (except at the Tractability tests, see above) because we
stopped the experimental work when the wolves reached the age of 2.
Before that age wolves did not attack either their caretaker or other
familiar experimenters, although they displayed agonistic behavior
several times towards them. Knowing well that hand-reared wolves can
pose a real threat to humans (e.g., Fox, 1971, pp. 102-109) handlers
tried to preclude conflicts if it was possible.
Attachment to the human caregiver. Earlier
work has established that dogs show attachment behavior to their
caregivers analogous to that of human toddlers in the Strange Situation
Test (Topál, Miklósi, Dóka, & Csányi, 1998). The original test
(Ainsworth & Wittig, 1969) consisted of several episodes of separation
and reunions between a mother and child. Based on this work, the
functional definition of attachment is that the subject is able to
discriminate and respond differentially to the object of attachment,
shows specific preference toward the caregiver (proximity and contact
seeking), and responds to separation from and reunion with the
attachment figure differently than to similar experiences with others
(see Gácsi, Topál, Miklósi, Dóka, & Csányi, 2001).
Dogs, like children, show preference for their
caregivers and emit specific greeting behavior in comparison between the
caregiver and a stranger (Topál et al., 1998; Gácsi et al., 2001;
Marston, Bennett, & Coleman, 2005). Comparing the attachment behavior of
our 4-month-old hand-reared wolves and dogs revealed marked differences.
Puppies showed an adult-like pattern of attachment behavior that can be
characterized by more proximity and contact seeking toward their
caregiver in comparison to an unfamiliar person, whereas such
discrimination was lacking in wolf cubs (Topál, Gácsi, et al., 2005;
Figure 6). This occurred despite the fact that the social experience
with humans was comparable for both species. If attachment behavior is
the outcome of social experience only, we would not expect a difference
between dogs and wolves. Thus the lack of attachment toward the primary
caregiver (who bottle-fed them and with whom they spent their first week
in close contact) in wolves is likely to have genetic basis. And by
similar logic, the attachment behavior of dogs is probably the result of
their evolutionary adaptation to the human niche.
Figure 6. Mean scores (+SE) for
following the owner and stranger who is leaving the
testing room (a; range 0-3) and for greeting the
entering owner and stranger (b; range 0-5). * indicates
significant difference (*p<.05, **p<.01). From "The
Effect of Domestication and Socialization on Attachment
to Human: A Comparative Study on Hand Reared Wolves and
Differently Socialized Dog Puppies," by J. Topál, M. Gácsi, Á. Miklósi, Zs. Virányi, E. Kubinyi, and V.
Csányi, 2005, Animal Behavior, 70, p. 1371. Copyright
2005 by Elsevier. Reprinted with permission of the
author. |
|
The lack of attachment in wolves does not mean that
wolves do not recognize and prefer their primary caregiver in comparison
to other familiar humans or an unfamiliar person. In a free interaction
situation, after longer separation from the caregiver (several days), we
observed the greeting behavior of the wolves. When different persons
entered the enclosure (one by one) the wolves approached their
caregivers sooner than others, wagged their tail more often in their
presence (note that at the age of 3-5 weeks we could not register tail
wagging on the same subjects in the object preference test, in contrast
to dogs). Also, wolves jumped up on their caregivers more frequently
compared to others. Interesting to mention here, and telling about the
memory capacities of wolves, they approached an unfamiliar person sooner
at their first meeting than at their second, although sometimes several
weeks passed between the two occasions (Virányi et al., 2002; Figure 7).
Figure 7. Greeting behavior of 1
year-old wolves. Mean (+SE) latency of approach of
different visitors entering the enclosure (a) and mean
(+SE) relative duration of tail wagging when
encountering different visitors. Different letters
indicate significant difference (p<0.05). |
|
Cooperation. Wolves are famous for their
cooperative ability when hunting large game in packs (Mech, 1970). They
employ a range of tactics to chase, catch, and kill their prey (Peters,
1978), although experimental modeling of these complex abilities is not
yet available. Interestingly, data are also scarce in the case of the
dog. It seems that the domestication process disrupted the ancient
social organisation. In feral dogs, paternal and alloparental care are
reduced; they do not hunt cooperatively, but prefer scavenging or
hunting small prey alone (Boitani et al., 1995; Butler, du Toit, &
Bingham, 2004). However, it is obvious that a gun dog or an assistant
dog shows cooperative behaviour in the context of the human-dog
relationship. In a descriptive study we found complex division of labor
when we observed the interaction between blind persons and their trained
guide dogs (Naderi, Miklósi, Dóka, & Csányi, 2001). When they navigate
together, certain tasks can be executed by one partner only. The human
knows the goal of the trip, and he/she might even go along a special
route. In contrast, only the dog receives visual information about the
actual state of the environment. A detailed behavioral analysis revealed
that human and dog alternatingly took on the role of initiation during
walking. For most of the time one partner led the other in one or two
subsequent actions and then the initialization was handed over to the
other partner. Such a finely tuned cooperative interaction between dogs
and humans, where the initialization of actions is exchanged
continuously, can be regarded as a functional equivalent of complex
forms of between-human cooperation characterized by Reynolds (1993) as
complementing.
Synchronizing function of behavior: Learning
by observation
Successful functioning of social groups often depends
on the ability of individuals to carry out actions in a coordinated way.
The human species is especially inventive from this point of view
because of our inclination for music and dance, and the ability of
empathy or hypnotizability are regarded to serve this function (Csányi,
2000). However, there are several behavioral mechanisms that support the
group-level synchrony in animals, for example, learning by observation.
Davey (1981) suggests that learning by observation facilitates social
cohesiveness. Social learning provides the advantage that behavioral
skills can be acquired with relatively little effort by observing a
knowledgeable individual (demonstrator). Synchronization is achieved at
the group level because social learning facilitates the distribution of
behavioral skills among the group members and between generations.
In spite of the fact that in the case of many social
mammals social learning has been shown to exist, very little is known
about the Canids. Although, in reviewing the situation from an
ecological perspective, Nel (1999) argued for the role of social
learning in these species, experimental evidence was not presented.
Frank (1980) cited some anecdotal evidence that wolves could find out
how to open latches of their cage by observing humans, in contrast to
the dogs in adjacent kennels, which could not do this. In his view, this
suggested that wolves are superior to dogs with regard to social
learning.
While there is still limited evidence of social
learning in wolves, recent experiments provided clear support for such
ability in dogs, which refute Frank et al.’s (1989) suggestions that dogs
are not able to learn by observation. Dogs that are exposed to a human
or conspecific demonstrator solving detour or instrumental tasks are
more successful at those tasks than dogs that have not witnessed the
demonstration (Pongrácz et al., 2001; Pongrácz, Miklósi, Tímár-Geng, &
Csányi, 2003; Pongrácz, Miklósi, Timár-Geng, & Csányi, 2004). Moreover,
dogs can use socially obtained information for overcoming their own
habitual action (Pongrácz, Miklósi, Kubinyi, Topál, & Csányi, 2003), or
adopt a virtually useless habit (Kubinyi, Miklósi, Topál, & Csányi,
2003) by following some pattern of human demonstrations, even in the
absence of any direct reward (Kubinyi, Topál, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2003).
More recently, some evidence has been obtained that dogs could be
capable of using a human behavior action as a cue for showing a
functionally similar behavior (Topál, Byrne, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2006)
and follow social rules in the context of interacting with humans
(Topál, Kubinyi, Gácsi, & Miklósi, 2005).
In sum, dogs proved to be very flexible in social
learning in that they are able to learn from (members of) another
species and do so without food or any other causal reinforcement, even
in cases where the goal or the result of the action is not clear. This
ability also emerges in human development (e.g., Meltzoff, 1996). It is
unfortunate that such experiments have not been carried out on
socialized wolves.
Constructivity: Communicative behavior
Understanding of human gestural signals.
Many assume that the increased communicative abilities of dogs
contributed to their successful integration into the human niche.
However, little attention was directed to the heterospecific aspects, as
experimental research was devoted mainly to intraspecific communication
(wolves: Fox, 1971; dogs: Bradshaw & Nott, 1995). Some researchers have
proposed that the ability of dogs to communicate with humans is mainly
based on the complex communicative skills of wolves. For example, wolves
often communicate with each other by pointing to a phenomenon of
interest, when, for example, they indicate the direction of prey for the
others (this behavior was probably selected for in pointers). The use of
such a directional cue could, in principle, facilitate the understanding
of human directional signals, such as pointing.
Recently, different experiments have been conducted
to compare the ability of socialized wolves and dogs to find hidden food
on the basis of human signals. Hare, Brown, Williamson, and Tomasello
(2002) tested 7 family dogs and 7 sanctuary-kept wolves’ reaction to
proximal pointing and gazing cues while standing in front of their cage.
In contrast to dogs, wolves did not rely on human gestures. However, we
should note that although the wolves were hand-reared, the littermates
had not been separated, and after the first two months they spent their
life in an enclosure. Their experiences were hardly comparable to those
of family dogs (see also Packard, 2003). The rearing history influences
the performance: Human-reared wolves outperformed mixed socialized
wolves (reared both by humans and a wolf) in a visual discrimination
task (Frank et al., 1989).
It is not surprising that our wolves, which were
socialized to humans at a comparable level to dogs, were able to find
food spontaneously on the basis of simple discriminative human cues
similar to those mentioned above. Wolves performed above chance if the
experimenter tapped the bowl, presented proximal pointing (when the
pointing finger is at 10 cm from the bowl), or stood behind the bowl
containing the food, even in the lack of gazing at the baited bowl in
all cases (Miklósi et al., 2003; Virányi et al., 2006a; Figure 8). These
results suggest that in Hare et al.’s study (2002) the low performance
of wolves could be due to their insufficient level of socialization.
Figure 8. Cumulative number
of Blocks, when wolves reached a significant level
of correct choice (p<0.05). One Block consisted of
20 trials; subjects had 2 Blocks per cueing type. |
|
Another important, but often neglected, aspect of
pointing tasks is that when the experimenter is pointing, the animal
needs to look at the human (body and face) instead of looking elsewhere.
Interestingly, even our extensively socialized wolves did not search for
eye contact with humans spontaneously either in this or in other
experimental situations (Virányi et al., 2006a; Miklósi et al., 2003).
This would partially explain why wolves performed relatively poorly with
more subtle gestures, such as momentary distal pointing. (In momentary
distal pointing, the experimenter’s pointing finger is more than 50 cm
from the bowl, and he/she moves his/her arm to the initial position
after pointing, so the subject cannot see the cue when approaching the
bowl. The experimenter looks at the animal and not at the bowl.)
Importantly, dog puppies as young as 4 months old are
able to perform well with the momentary distal pointing, without any
special, intensive, and early socialization to humans. Although the
everyday life of dogs provides many opportunities to learn about human
gestures, dogs proved to rely on the distal pointing cue at the same
level at the ages of 4 and 11 months.
After extensive training, wolves significantly
improved in parallel with increased readiness to look at the pointing
human. At the age of 11 months, wolves reached the performance level of
same aged, naïve dogs (Virányi et al., 2006a; Figure 9). It is important
that wolves showed large inter-individual variations in their
performance. This high variability could be the base of selection for
improved communicative abilities toward humans that dogs seem to be
strongly selected for. In human evolution, communicative behaviour had a
special importance (Donald, 1991; Csányi, 2000); thus it is probable
that the domestication process affected dogs’ ability to communicate
with humans. In fact, although there is still debate about whether
chimpanzees understand the pointing gesture (Miklósi & Soproni, 2006),
it is now known that dogs can reach similar levels of success at
pointing as 1.5-2-year-old children (Lakatos, Soproni, Dóka, & Miklósi,
submitted manuscript). Dogs are able to respond to highly various forms
of pointing gestures, such as pointing with one’s leg (Soproni, Miklósi,
Topál, & Csányi, 2002) and are able to recognize minute behavioral cues
characterizing human visual attention (Call, Bräuer, Kaminski, &
Tomasello, 2003; Gácsi, Miklósi, Varga, Topál, & Csányi, 2004; Virányi,
Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2004).
Figure 9. Mean number of correct
choices (+SE) (a) and mean latency of getting and
maintaining eye contact with the pointing experimenter
(+SE) (b) in a two-way choice task of 4-month-old pet
dogs, hand-reared dogs, and hand-reared wolves. Mean
number of correct choices (+SE) (c) and mean latency of
getting and maintaining eye contact with the pointing
experimenter (+SE) (d) in a two choice task of
11-month-old naïve pet dogs and trained hand-reared
wolves. Dotted line indicates random choice. * indicates
significant difference (p<0.05). From "Comprehension of
Human Pointing Gestures in Young Human-Reared Wolves (Canis
Lupus) and Dogs (Canis familiaris)," by Zs. Virányi, M. Gácsi, E. Kubinyi, J. Topál, B. Belényi, D. Ujfalussy,
and Á. Miklósi, 2006, Manuscript submitted for
publication. Reprinted with permission of the author. |
|
Initiation of communicative interactions.
Discussing the understanding of human gestures, we noted that wolves had
decreased willingness to look at humans even after intensive
socialization. This is not the case in dogs, which look at humans in
cases of uncertainty (Topál, Miklósi, & Csányi, 1997) and when they are
physically constrained from obtaining hidden food (Miklósi, Polgárdi,
Topál, & Csányi, 2000). Wolves in this latter situation alternated their
gaze significantly less between the location of food and their caretaker
than dogs did (Virányi et al., 2006b).
The result that wolves are not ready to initiate
communicative interactions with humans gained more support from other
comparative experiments. We designed a test situation consisting of two
phases. In the first phase the subjects learned how to obtain a piece of
meat by pulling a rope. Importantly, there was no difference either in
the speed of learning or in the orientation of scratching and sniffing
the apparatus between the two species (in contrast to Frank & Frank,
1982). For the second phase the animals were prevented from obtaining
the reward because the rope was fastened imperceptibly to the frame of
the apparatus. We wanted to know whether there is any difference in the
behavior of dogs and wolves in such an insolvable situation. The
analysis found that after some unsuccessful attempts the dogs looked at
the human caretaker standing near to them, but the wolves ignored the
human, kept on trying on their own, or gave up and had a rest. The
results were the same when a food item was in a covered litter bin, and
after some successful trials, we attached the lid to the container in
such a way that it could not be opened (Miklósi et al., 2003; Figure
10). Frank and Frank (1985, p. 271) also noted the difference between
the two species in following gazing signals: "The wolves generally
attacked each puzzle immediately upon release from the start box and
persisted until either the problem was solved or time had run out. In
contrast, the malamutes investigated puzzle boxes only until they
discovered that the food was not easily accessible, after which they
typically returned to the start box and performed a variety of
solicitation and begging gestures toward Experimenter 1."
Figure 10. Median (sec) +
interquartile range of latency of gazing at the
owner both at the bin- and rope-task (a) and
relative duration of gazing at the owner both at the
bin- and rope-task (b) in normally raised pet dogs
and hand-reared wolves at the 16-week-old age from
Miklósi et al. (2003, Figure 2-3., pp. 764-765.).
Boxes indicate the 50% of the data (lower and upper
interquartile range). Whiskers extend to the
smallest and largest values excluding outliers and
extremities. Dogs look significantly sooner
(p<0.025) and longer at the owners than wolves did
(p<0.056). From "A Simple Reason for a Big
Difference: Wolves do not Look Back at Humans, but
Dogs do," by Á. Miklósi, E. Kubinyi, J. Topál, M.
Gácsi, Zs. Virányi, and V. Csányi, 2003, Current
Biology, 13, pp. 764-765. Copyright 2003 by
Elsevier. Reprinted with permission of the author. |
|
In a preliminary study we observed readiness for eye
contact by means of operant conditioning. Subjects at the age of 5 and 9
weeks were rewarded immediately with a food pellet if they established
eye contact with a familiar experimenter. Five-week-old wolves, although
motivated by food, fell asleep during the 4-min test-session: a strategy
they employed for dealing with unsolvable problems in other tests as
well. Four weeks later, the wolves made intensive attempts to reach the
plate containing the food as the session went on, but the frequency of
their gazing at the experimenter did not change. In contrast, dogs
established eye contact with the experimenter significantly more
frequently during the fourth minute (Gácsi et al., 2005).
Subsequent tests on our intensively socialized wolves
and dogs confirmed that dogs appear to have an innate readiness to look
at human faces, whereas wolves seem to ignore the human gaze. We
observed the behavior of 9-week-old wolf and dog puppies called to a
closed coop containing fresh chicken meat. The caretaker, standing at 3
m from the coop, called the animal by name. Dogs looked at their
caretaker sooner than wolves. Both species’ motivation for obtaining the
food was probably similar, since both dogs and wolves orientated to the
coop for about same amount of time. We repeated the test at the age of 6
months, but now the caretakers were not allowed to talk to the animals
and stood motionless 2 m from the coop for 2 minutes. Again, dogs looked
at their caretakers sooner and for longer than wolves. However, wolves
orientated more to the coop than dogs did. At the time of testing wolves
already lived together in a pack for more than 2 months, which probably
had an effect on their competitiveness, motivation, and endurance
(Újváry, 2004).
Two nonexclusive processes might account for the
difference between dogs’ and wolves’ willingness to look at a human
face. Dogs may have been selected for an inclination to look at humans.
Also, or alternatively, dogs may tolerate being gazed at by humans
better than wolves. Extended gazing by humans may act as a threat to
wolves (see Vas, Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2005) which induced
subordinate or agonistic behavior (Fox, 1971; Schenkel, 1967).
In human communication, the dynamics of eye (or face)
contact is understood as initialization and maintenance of a
communicative interaction (Gomez, 1996). We assume that humans selected
for this human-like communicative behavior in the ancestors of modern
dogs. This relatively subtle change in behavior could have crucial
consequences, as it provides a potential starting point for the
integration of dog and human communication systems (Miklósi et al.,
2003, p. 765). In other words, looking at our face, a dog can modify our
behavior in a way that is advantageous for the animal. This behavior can
be interpreted as social tool use (Gomez, 1996).
Figure 11. Mean (+SE) of
relative durations of time spent with vocalization at
the age of 3, 4, and 5 weeks in object-preference tests
from Gácsi et al., 2005 (Figure 4, p. 118). Dogs spent
more time vocalizing than wolves, with no effect of age
(p<0.01). Vocalization decreased with age in the case of
dogs, while wolves’ vocalization did not change with age
(p<0.05). From "Species-Specific Differences and
Similarities in the Behavior of Hand Raised Dog and Wolf
Puppies in Social Situations with Humans," by M. Gácsi,
B. Gyri, Á. Miklósi, Zs. Virányi, E. Kubinyi, J. Topál,
and V. Csányi, 2005, Developmental Psychobiology, 47,
p. 118. Copyright 2005 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Reprinted with Permission of the author. |
|
Vocal
communication. Barking is one of the most
striking features of dog behavior, yet it has received little attention.
Ethological observations has provided evidence that barking is also part
of the wolf’s vocal repertoire (e.g., Schassburger, 1993), but it is
used only with a very restricted meaning of warning or protesting with
relatively little variation in acoustic structure (Feddersen-Pettersen,
2000). In contrast, dogs bark in a variety of different situations and
produce a highly variable vocalization (Fox, 1971; Feddersen-Pettersen,
2000; Yin, 2002). Quantitative differences in vocalizations start at an
early age. Observing 3-5-week-old human-fostered wolf cubs and dog
puppies in a series of object-preference tests revealed that puppies
spent more time vocalizing during the tests than cubs did. However, we
never observed growling or barking. All vocalizations were high pitched
sounds (e.g., whining or yelping), mainly when the animals were not in
the proximity of the objects. We infer that the behavior is a sign of
distress (Gácsi et al., 2005; Figure 11).
Earlier theories of dog vocalization suggested that
it simply represents a hypertrophied behavior which was a by-product of
domestication, but recent studies have shown that the situation might be
more complex. Human subjects exposed to bark sequences recorded in
different behavioral contexts (e.g., a dog is tethered to a tree,
attacks a stranger through a fence, or plays with its caretaker) were
able to categorize these contexts correctly (significantly above chance)
when provided with a list of possibilities (Pongrácz, Molnár, Miklósi, &
Csányi, 2005; Figure 12 and 13). In a subsequent test, subjects
indicated the emotional content of the bark using a list of adjectives.
For example, barks recorded when the dog attacked the stranger received
the highest scores on aggressiveness. The analysis of the barks revealed
that various acoustic parameters correlated with the assumed emotional
content. Barks recorded in aggressive situations were characterized by
lower fundamental frequency and reduced harmonic content with a shorter
inter-bark interval. Affiliative vocalizations had a higher fundamental
frequency and included more harmonics (Pongrácz et al., 2005; Figure
12). This suggests that selective processes during their socialization
among humans appears to have favored dogs able to signal their situation
and their emotional states. This change in the use of barking seems to
correspond to the structural rules suggested by Morton (1977).
These hypothesized changes in barking together with
similar changes in the visual communicative system of dogs suggest a
trend toward an increase in the number and kinds of communicative
behaviors in dogs. Such increased flexibility in communicative behavior,
which was also noted by Frank (1980), would be highly adaptive in
forming complex communicative interactions with humans.
Figure 12. Spectrograms of
barks from a Mudi dog when: tethered (top), playing
(middle) and a stranger approaches (bottom). |
|
Overview on the Wolf-Dog Comparative Data
Figure 13. Mean percentage (+SE)
of correctly categorized situation by humans who
listened dog barks from several contexts. Different
letters indicate significant differences. From "Human
Listeners Classify Dog Barks in Different Situations,"
by P. Pongrácz, Cs. Molnár, Á. Miklósi, and V. Csányi,
2005, Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, p. 143.
Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological
Association. Reprinted with permission of the author. |
|
Raising wolf cubs and dog puppies in an identical way
revealed many specific social behavioral differences between the two
species, especially with regard to their interactions with humans. Even
at an early age (3-5 weeks), dogs displayed more communicative signals
(e.g., vocalization, tail wagging, gazing at the human’s face) and were
less aggressive and avoidant than wolves, although the general activity
level did not differ between the two species (Gácsi et al., 2005).
Due to human fostering, 5-week-old wolves showed a
clear preference for their caregiver in an object preference test, if
the other stimulus-object was another human (Gácsi et al., 2005; Figure
4). However, in contrast to dogs, wolves’ preferences for the caregiver
did not develop into a behavioral pattern that could be categorized as
attachment. Hand-reared dogs and pet dogs, but not individually
socialized, hand-reared wolves, exhibited highly different
responsiveness to their caregiver compared to an unfamiliar human as
early as at the age of 16 weeks (Topál, Gácsi, et al., 2005). While
wolves did not display characteristic patterns of attachment toward
their caregiver, their preference for her remained strong at the age of
1 or 2 years (Virányi et al., 2002).
Many assume that domestication affected dogs’ ability
to communicate with humans. Wolves, given that their socialization is
comparable to that of dogs, were able to follow human cues that have a
local enhancement or food-hand association component (e.g., touching,
proximal pointing; Miklósi et al., 2003; Virányi et al., 2006a). Recent
results for farm-reared foxes not selected or trained at approaching
humans (Hare et al., 2005), show that they too were able to follow human
proximal pointing and gazing (Hare et al., 2005). In this context it is
important to recall that our hand–reared dogs, but not wolves, were able
to use more difficult human pointing gestures (e.g., momentary distal
pointing) spontaneously and that wolves needed more training to reach
the same level of success that dogs reached instantly. The reason for
this difference might be that in contrast to dogs it was very difficult
to establish gaze-to-gaze contact with the wolves; therefore, wolves
were less able to attend to an experimenter’s gestures for an extended
duration (Miklósi et al., 2003; Gácsi et al., 2005; Virányi et al.,
2006a). Dogs are inclined to look at our faces, and this inclination
provides them with a broadened opportunity for learning about human
gestures.
Finally, we suggest that future researchers observing
wolves in human-animal interactions should follow our lead. Wolves
should receive extensive human contact individually beginning before the
age of 10 days, for more than 20 hours a day, and should not be exposed
to conspecifics early in life. Also, we believe that our insistence on
early, extensive, and identical training in wolves and dogs make it
likely that the differences we have observed are the result of species
rather than experiential differences.
Dogs as the Model of Socio-Cognitive Behavior in Humans
According to Hare and Tomasello (2005), the major
cause of behavioral differences between humans and apes is a basic
change in the temperament. Selection against overt aggressive behavior,
which had prevented the execution of complex cooperative actions in our
ancestors, was the important change. This hypothesis is based on the
observation that some animals, selected artificially for tameness (e.g.,
foxes, see Belyaev, 1978) show increased performance in understanding
human cues (Hare & Tomasello, 2005). Although such changes may have
contributed to the important behavioral differences between other
primates and early human groups, we have argued that the evidence is too
scant as yet to evaluate the role of temperament in human evolution
(Miklósi & Topál, 2005; see also Byrne, 2005). We have proposed an
alternative view that human evolution had an effect on not one, but many
features of social behavior in a complex interacting way, during a
stepwise process (Topál, Miklósi, et al., 2006; Byrne, 2005). These
changes can be traced in the Human Behavior Complex (HBC) model which
lists features of human social behavior that are assumed to have
undergone alterations after the Pan-Homo split. Parallel changes in a
different species could show that this model is viable. We think that
dogs can provide a model because there seem to be a number of
overlapping components in HBC and DBC, making dogs a fruitful
comparative model for early social behavioral evolution in humans.
We assume that divergence of dogs from wolves
represents evolutionary steps taken in the same direction of increased
sociability and cooperative and communicative abilities as observed in
the case of the Pan-Homo clade. In our view, the comparative analysis of
dogs and wolves should help scientists understand the first steps taken
by our ancestors towards recent humans.
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Wittig, B. A. (1969).
Attachment and exploratory behavior of one-year olds in a strange
situation. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behaviour
(pp. 111-136). London: Meuthen.
Altman, D. (1987). Social behavior patterns in
three wolf packs at Tierpark Berlin. In H. Frank (Ed.), Man and
wolf: Advances, issues and problems in captive wolf research
(pp. 415-424). Dordrecht: W. Junk Publishers.
Belyaev, D. K. (1978). Destabilizing selection as
a factor in domestication. Journal of Heredity, 70, 301-308.
Berryman, J. C., Howells, K., & Lloyd-Evans, M.
(1985). Pet owner attitudes to pets and people: A psychological
study. The Veterinary Record, 117, 659-661.
Boitani, C., Francisci, F., & Ciucci, P. (1995).
Population biology and ecology of feral dogs in central Italy. In J.
Serpell (Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behavior and
interactions with people (pp. 218-245). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Bradshaw, J. W. S., & Nott, H. M. R. (1995).
Social and communication behavior of companion dogs. In J. Serpell
(Ed.), The domestic dog: Its evolution, behavior and interactions
with people (pp. 115-130). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Butler, J. R. A., du Toit, J. T., & Bingham, J.
(2004). Free ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as
predators and prey in rural Zimbabwe: Threats of competition and
disease to large wild carnivores. Biological Conservation, 115,
369-378.
Byrne, R. W. (2005). Animal evolution: Foxy
friends. Current Biology, 15, R86-87.
Cain, A. O. (1983). A study of pets in the family
system. In A. H. Katcher & A. M. Beck (Eds.), New perspectives on
our lives with companion animals (pp. 72-81). Philadelphia:
University of Philadelphia Press.
Cairns, R. B., & Werboff, J. (1967). Behavior
development in the dog: An interspecific analysis. Science, 158,
1070-1072.
Call, J., Bräuer, J., Kaminski, J., & Tomasello,
M. (2003). Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to
the attentional state of humans. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 117, 257-263.
Cohen, S. P. (2002). Can pets function as family
members? Western Journal of Nursing Research, 24, 621-638.
Coppinger, R. P., & Coppinger, L. (2001). Dogs.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Csányi, V. (2000). The ‘human behavior complex’
and the compulsion of communication: Key factors in human evolution.
Semiotica, 128, 45-60.
Davey, G. (1981). Animal learning and
conditioning. London: Macmillan Education.
Davis, S. J. M., & Valla, F. R. (1978). Evidence
for domestication of the dog 12,000 years ago in the Natufian of
Israel. Nature, 276, 608–610.
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Feddersen-Petersen, D. (1991). The ontogeny of
social play and antagonistic behavior in selected canid species.
Bonner Zoologische Beitraege, 42, 97-114.
Feddersen-Petersen, D. (2000). Vocalization of
European wolf (Canis lupus lupus L.) and various dog breeds (Canis
lupus f. fam.). Archieves für Tierzüchtung (Dummerstorf), 43,
387-397.
Fentress, J. C. (1967). Observations on the
behavioral development of a hand-reared male timber wolf.
American Zoologist, 7, 339-351.
Finlayson, C. (2005). Biogeography and evolution
of the genus Homo. Trends in Evolution and Ecology, 20,
457-463.
Fox, M. W. (1970). Behavioral effects of rearing
dogs with cats during the ‘critical period’ of socialisation.
Behavior, 35, 273-280.
Fox, M. W. (1971). Behavior of wolves, dogs
and related canids. London: The Trinity Press.
Fox, M. W. (1972). Socioecological implications
of individual differences in wolf litters: A developmental and
evolutionary perspective. Behavior, 45, 298-313.
Fox, M. W. (1973). Social dynamics of three
captive wolf packs. Behavior, 47, 290-301.
Fox, M. W., & Stelzner, D. (1966).
Approach/withdrawal variables in the development of social behavior
in the dog. Animal Behavior, 14, 362-366.
Frank, A. (1987). Man and wolf: Advances, issues and problems in
captive wolf research. Dordrecht: W. Junk Publishers.
Frank, H. (1980). Evolution of canine information
processing under conditions of natural and artificial selection.
Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5, 389-399.
Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1982). Comparison of
problem-solving performance in six-week-old wolves and dogs.
Animal Behaviour, 30, 95-98.
Frank, H., & Frank, M. G. (1985). Comparative
manipulation-test performance in ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus)
and Alaskan malamutes (C. familiaris): A Piagetian
interpretation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99,
266-274.
Frank, H., Frank, M. G., Hasselbach, L. M., &
Littleton, D. M. (1989). Motivation and insight in wolf (Canis
lupus) and Alaskan malamute (Canis familiaris): Visual
discrimination learning. Bulletin of Psychonomic Society, 27,
455-458.
Freedman, D. G., King, J. A., & Elliot, O.
(1961). Critical period in the social development of dogs.
Science, 133, 1016–1017.
Gácsi, M., Gyri, B., Miklósi, Á., Virányi, Zs.,
Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., et al. (2005). Species-specific differences
and similarities in the behavior of hand raised dog and wolf puppies
in social situations with humans. Developmental Psychobiology, 47,
111-122.
Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Varga, O., Topál, J., &
Csányi, V. (2004). Are readers of our face readers of our minds?
Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition
of human’s attention. Animal Cognition, 7, 144-153.
Gácsi, M., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., &
Csányi, V. (2001). Attachment behavior of adult dogs (Canis
familiaris) living at rescue centres: Forming new bonds.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115, 423-431.
Gomez, J. C. (1996). Ostensive behaviour in great
apes: The role of eye contact. In A. E. Russon, S. T. Parker, & K.
Bard (Eds.), Reaching into thought (pp. 131-151). Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Gomez, J. C. (2005). Species comparative studies
and cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9,
118-125.
Grzimek, B. (1942). Weitere Vergleichsversuche
mit Wolf und Hund. Zeitschrift für vergleichende Physiologie, 5,
59–73.
Gyri, B. (2004). Social attraction and
communication in hand-raised wolves and dogs. Unpublished
master’s thesis, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary.
Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., & Tomasello,
M. (2002). The domestication of cognition in dogs. Science, 298,
1634-1636.
Hare, B., Plyusnina, I., Ignacio, N., Schepina,
O., Stepika, A., Wrangham, R., et al. (2005). Social cognitive
evolution in captive foxes is a correlated by-product of
experimental domestication. Current Biology, 15, 226–230.
Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like
social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9,
439-444.
Hemmer, H. (1990). Domestication. The decline
of environmental appreciation. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Klinghammer, E., & Goodmann, P. A. (1987).
Socialization and management of wolves in captivity. In H. Frank
(Ed.), Man and wolf: Advances, issues and problems in captive
wolf research (pp. 31-60). Dordrecht: W. Junk Publishers.
Kubinyi, E., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V.
(2003). Social anticipation in dogs: A new form of social influence.
Animal Cognition, 6, 57-64.
Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V.
(2003). Dogs learn from their owner via observation in a
manipulation task. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117,
156-165.
Lakatos, G., Soproni, K., Dóka, A., & Miklósi, Á.
(2006). A comparative approach of how dogs and human infants are
able to utilize various forms of pointing gestures. Manuscript
submitted for publication.
Marston, L. C., Bennett, P. C., & Coleman, G. J.
(2005). Factors affecting the formation of a canine-human bond.
IWDBA Conference Proceedings, 132-138.
Mech, L. D. (1970). The wolf: The ecology and
behavior of an endangered species. New York: Natural History.
Mech, L. D. (1999). Alpha status, dominance, and
division of labor in wolf packs. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 77,
1196–1203.
Mech, L. D., & Boitani, L. (Eds.). (2003).
Wolves: Ecology, behavior and conservation. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). The human infant as
imitative generalist: A 20-year progress report on infant imitation
with implications for comparative psychology. In I. C. M. Heyes & B.
G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture
(pp. 347-370). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Gácsi, M.,
Virányi, Zs., & Csányi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big
difference: Wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do.
Current Biology, 13, 763-766.
Miklósi Á., Polgárdi, R., Topál, J., & Csányi, V.
(2000). Intentional behavior in dog-human communication: An
experimental analysis of ‘showing’ behavior in the dog. Animal
Cognition, 3, 159-166.
Miklósi, Á., & Soproni, K. (2006). Comprehension
of the human pointing gesture in animals: A comparative approach.
Animal Cognition, 9, 81-93.
Miklósi, Á., & Topál, J. (2005). Is there a
simple recipe for how to make friends? Commentary. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 9, 463-464.
Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2004).
Comparative social cognition: What can dogs teach us? Animal
Behavior, 67, 995-1004.
Mitchell, R.W. (2001). Americans’ talk to dogs
during play: Similarities and differences with talk to infants.
Research on Language and Social Interaction, 34, 182–210.
Morey, D. F. (2006). Burying key evidence: The
social bond between dogs and people. Journal of Archaeological
Science, 33, 178-185.
Morton, E. S. (1977). On the occurrence and
significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal
sounds. American Naturalist, 111, 855-869.
Musiani, M., Mamo, C., Boitani, L., Callaghan,
C., Gates, C.C., Mattei, L., et al. (2003). Wolf depredation trends
and the use of fladry barriers to protect livestock in western North
America. Conservation Biology, 17, 1538-1547.
Naderi, Sz., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V.
(2001). Cooperative interactions between blind persons and their
dog. Applied Animal Behavior Sciences, 74, 59-80.
Nel, J. A. J. (1999). Social learning in canids:
An ecological perspective. In H. O. Box & K. R. Gibson (Eds.),
Mammalian social learning (pp. 259-277). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Nobis, G. (1979). Der älteste Haushund lebte vor
14000 Jahren. Umschau in Wissenschaft und Technik, 19, 610.
Packard, J. M. (2003). Wolf behavior:
Reproductive, social and intelligent. In L. D. Mech & L. Boitani
(Eds.), Wolves: Behavior, ecology and conservation (pp.
35-65). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Peters, R. (1978). Communication, cognitive
mapping and strategy in wolves and hominids. In R. L. Hall & H. S.
Sharp (Eds.), Wolf and man, evolution in parallel (pp.
95-107). New York: Academic Press.
Plyusnina, I., Oskina, I., & Trut, L. (1991). An
analysis of fear and aggression during early development of behavior
in silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Applied Animal Behaviour
Science, 32, 253–268.
Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Gurobi,
K., Topál, J., & Csányi, V. (2001). Social learning in dogs: The
effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a
detour task. Animal Behaviour, 62, 1109-1117.
Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Kubinyi, E., Topál,
J., & Csányi, V. (2003). Interaction between individual experience
and social learning in dogs. Animal Behaviour, 65, 595-603.
Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Timár-Geng, K., &
Csányi, V. (2003). Preference for copying unambiguous demonstrations
in dogs (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 117, 337-343.
Pongrácz, P., Miklósi, Á., Timár-Geng, K., &
Csányi, V. (2004). Verbal attention getting as a key factor in
social learning between dog (Canis familiaris) and human.
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 375-383.
Pongrácz, P., Molnár, Cs., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi,
V. (2005). Human listeners classify dog barks in different
situations. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 136-144.
Rabb, G. B., Woolpy, J. H., & Ginsburg, B. E.
(1967). Social relationships in a group of captive wolves.
American Zoologist, 7, 305-312.
Reynolds, P.C. (1993). The complementation theory
of language and tool use. In K. R. Gibson & T. Ingold (Eds.),
Tool use, language and cognition in human evolution (pp.
407-428). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Savolainen, P., Zhang, Y., Ling, J., Lundeberg,
J., & Leitner, T. (2002). Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin
of domestic dogs. Science, 298, 610-613.
Schassburger, R. M. (1993). Vocal
communication in the timber wolf, Canis Lupus, Linnaeus: Structure,
motivation, and ontogeny. Berlin: Paul Parey Scientific
Publishers.
Schenkel, R. (1967). Submission: Its features and
function in the wolf and dog. American Zoologist, 7, 319-329.
Scott, J. P., & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics
and the social behavior of the dog. Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Soproni, K., Miklósi, Á., Topál, J., & Csányi, V.
(2002). Dogs’ (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human
pointing gestures. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 116,
27-34.
Topál, J., Byrne, R., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V.
(2006). Reproducing actions and action sequences: Do as I do in a
dog. Animal Cognition, 9, 355-367.
Topál, J. Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., Virányi, Zs.,
Kubinyi, E., & Csányi, V. (2005). The effect of domestication and
socialization on attachment to human: A comparative study on hand
reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Animal
Behavior, 70, 1367-1375.
Topál, J., Kubinyi, E., Gácsi, M., & Miklósi, Á.
(2005). Obeying social rules: A comparative study on dogs and
humans. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 3,
213-237.
Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (1997).
Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog.
Anthrozoös, 10, 214-224.
Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Dóka, A., & Csányi, V.
(1998). Attachment behavior in dogs: A new application of the
Ainsworth Strange Situation Test. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 112, 219-229.
Topál, J., Miklósi, Á., Gácsi, M., Dóka, A.,
Pongrácz, P., Kubinyi, E., et al. (2006). An alternative functional
model for the evolutionary study of human social behavior: The dog.
Manuscript submitted for publication.
Ujfalussy, D., Virányi, Zs., & Kubinyi, E.
(2003). Individual and intensive hand-rearing of wolves: The method
and its results in handling the wolves. ASAB Summer Meeting,
Grünau, Austria.
Újváry, D. (2004). Initiating communicative
interactions with humans. Comparative analysis between dogs and
wolves. Unpublished master’s thesis, Eötvös University,
Budapest, Hungary.
Vas, J., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á., &
Csányi, V. (2005). A friend or an enemy? Dogs’ reaction to an
unfamiliar person showing behavioural cues of threat and
friendliness at different times. Applied Animal Behaviour
Science, 94, 99-115.
Vilá, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonado, J. E.,
Amorom, I. R., Rice, J. E., Honeycutt, R. L., et al. (1997).
Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science, 276,
1687-1689.
Virányi, Zs., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Kurys, A.,
Miklósi, Á., & Csányi, V. (2002). Wolf-human interactions: Flight,
approach and greeting behavior toward familiar and unfamiliar humans
in hand-reared wolf pups (Canis lupus). Advances in
Ethology (Supplements to Ethology), 37, 83.
Virányi, Zs, Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J.,
Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., et al. (2006a). Comprehension of
human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus)
and dogs (Canis familiaris). Manuscript submitted for
publication.
Virányi, Zs, Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J.,
Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., et al. (2006b). Comparative analysis
of ‘showing’ behavior of human-reared dogs and wolves.
Unpublished manuscript, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary.
Virányi, Zs., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, Á.,
& Csányi, V. (2004). Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’
attentional focus. Behavioural Processes, 66, 161-172.
Yin, S. (2002). A new perspective on barking in
dogs. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 189-193.
Zimen, E. (1987). Ontogeny of approach and flight
behavior towards humans in wolves, poodles and wolf–poodle hybrids.
In H. Frank (Ed.), Man and wolf: Advances, issues and problems in
captive wolf research (pp. 275–292). Dordrecht: W. Junk
Publishers.
|
|