All material on this site is (c) copyright to the respective authors.  ISSN - 1481-3440 


American Indian Demographic History and Cultural
Affiliation: A Discussion of Certain Limitations on the Use
of mtDNA and Y Chromosome Testing
By
Peter N. Jones
The Bäuu Institute
PO Box 4445
USA
pnj@bauuinstitute.com

Page 2
1
Abstract
Mitochondrial DNA (mtDNA) and Y chromosome studies have been used increasingly over the
last 20 years by anthropological geneticists and others to reconstruct the peopling of the
Americas as well as to infer American Indian cultural affiliation and demographic histories.
While the promise of this method is great, there are several problems inherent in some of its
current uses. These limitations are discussed concerning the following six currently accepted
methods: interpretation of coalescent times as times of origin; 2) the current uses of haplogroups;
3) sample sizes; 4) use of language groups to define population groups; 5) use of contemporary
American Indian reservations to infer prehistoric tribal history; and 6) a combination of these to
determine American Indian population history, historic migrations, and demographic history.
This paper concludes that caution must be exercised in claiming too much for the method.
Instead, it is recommend that it be used in conjunction with other established sources of data
such as oral history, ethnography, linguistics, and archaeology when attempting to reconstruct
American Indian cultural history or in determining the cultural affiliation of groups.
Key words
Genetic anthropology, haplogroups, demographics, cultural affiliation, American Indians

Page 3
2
Introduction
Anthropological genetics is one of the more recent sub-fields in the study of American
Indian cultural affiliation and demographic history, dating back some 60 years (see Matson and
Schrader 1933; Matson 1938). However, in the last 20 years advances in technology have
allowed anthropological geneticists to explore the origin of modern humans, the size and
geographic origin of human populations, as well as the possibility of finding ethnic or even
individual people’s homelands. Recent articles using genetic data have claimed to link
Europe/Western Asian populations with North American Indian populations as having recent
common ancestry (Brown et al. 1998), identified a single wave of migration for the peopling of
the New World (i.e., Bianchi et al. 1997; Easton et al. 1996; Merriwether et al. 1995) as opposed
to several (i.e., Karafet et al. 1997), and concluded that some American Indian tribes recently
moved into a geographic area (Kaestle 1997; Kaestle and Smith 2001) despite contrary evidence
from oral history and archaeology. Some of the most publicized uses of genetic anthropology in
recent years concern the question of the peopling of the Americas, and in compliance with the
Native American Graves Protection and Repatriation Act (NAGPRA), as seen in such cases as
the Spirit Cave Mummy and the Kennewick Man repatriation controversies. In these two
examples the situation is complicated by the great antiquity of the skeletons, 9,415+/-25 years
ago and 8410+/-60 years ago, respectively (Napton 1997; Chatters 2000). The potential benefits
this new genetic research has to offer are vast and highly valuable. Such benefits include a better
understanding of the genetic and evolutionary factors that influence populations; an
understanding of maternally transmitted diseases such as blindness, epilepsy, dementias, cardiac
and skeletal muscle diseases, diabetes mellitus, and movement disorders; the development of
new metabolic and genetic therapies for mitochondrial diseases; and a better understanding of

Page 4
3
the geographic origin of anatomically modern humans, to name just a few. However, caution is
urged in applying the methods and results of present genetic research to the use of American
Indian cultural affiliation and demographic histories. This paper examines six weaknesses
inherent in current uses of genetic anthropology that attempt to resolve questions of demographic
history and prehistoric cultural affiliation: 1) interpretation of coalescent times as times of origin;
2) the current uses of haplogroups; 3) sample sizes; 4) use of language groups to define
population groups; 5) use of contemporary American Indian reservations to infer prehistoric
tribal history; and 6) a combination of these to determine American Indian population history,
historic migrations, and demographic history.
I. Coalescent Times as Times of Origin
A first problem confounding the uses of mtDNA (mitochondrial deoxyribonucleic acid)
and Y chromosomes to infer American Indian demographic histories is in interpreting the
coalescent times of genes as times of origin for the population. Although tracing the genealogy
of mtDNA theoretically can lead to a single common ancestor, this is not evidence that the
human population went through a period when only one breeding population was alive and
reproducing. Tracing the coalescent times leads to one ancestor of a unilineally transmitted set
of markers, but the descendents of the original mtDNA will have had haplotype frequencies that
differed among themselves, resulting in a biased sample of the total historic population when
using coalescent times. This is so because working back in time is does not allow one to take
into account the various branches of diversity that the historic population had, but only can detect
the lineal history of the specific marker being coalesced. Three primary assumptions arising

Page 5
4
from the use of coalescent times (Hoelzer et al. 1998; Hudson 1990; Templeton 1993; Wolpoff
1999) that have been employed in understanding American Indian demographic history are:
A) gene coalescence is a regular process of mutation accumulation in neutral systems,
and therefore can be timed like a regularly ticking clock with an acceptable range of
error,
B) American Indian populations were isolated from each other after they originated or
migrated to the Americas, and
C) the history of particular gene systems is the history of the specific populations in
which they are found.
Prior to the historic period, and especially before the formation of reservations beginning
in the 1850s, many American Indian groups were highly mobile autonomous entities, covering
large areas of land. Similarly, many American Indians practiced a high degree of spousal
exchange and intergroup marriage among other groups in order to solidify trade arrangements
and political alliances. Some of these exchanges took place well over 500 miles from where the
group has been historically recorded to inhabit. Examples of these trade centers are the large
Native fisheries of the Northwest Coast such as The Dalles, Celilo Falls, and the Lillooet River
Fishery (Schuster 1998; Stern 1998; Hayden 1992) where groups from the Northwest Coast,
Plateau, Northern Plains, and Great Basin regions gathered. Other examples can be found from
the archaeological record that show similar large regional centers that may have acted as
gathering and/or redistribution centers such as Chaco Canyon in the US southwest (Lekson
2000) and Monte Alban, San Jose Mogote, Tlapacoya, and Tlatilco in central Mexico (Flannery
and Marcus 1994). In fact, as Walker (1998:5-6) has noted for the Plateau peoples, “It is clear
that Plateau peoples were and remain highly inter-active maintaining extensive intergroup

Page 6
5
connections as well as extensive linkages with the Plains, Northwest Coast, and Great Basin
groups. Connections with Subarctic groups are evident in the northern reaches of the Plateau of
Canada.”
One important requirement in the coalescence theory is the use of random samples of
genes from the population under study. However, most studies have not used random samples,
but instead have used convenience samples obtained from diabetic studies, rheumatic studies,
and AIDS studies, as well as other studies (Jones, in preparation; also see below). As Donnelly
and Tavare (1995: 418) point out, “In practice, genetic data are typically obtained from
convenience samples rather than proper random samples. There is an obvious danger that such
data may contain individuals who share relatively too much ancestry on the relevant timescales.
The extent to which application of coalescent (or traditional) methods to such convenience
samples may be misleading remains an open, and potentially serious, question.” Furthermore,
most studies rely on the idea that American Indians came over in small groups (usually thought
to have occurred as part of one to three migration waves; see Dillehay 2000 to cite one recent
work) across the Bering Land Bridge in prehistoric times. If this is the case, coalescence times
will be shorter because smaller populations in the past are more likely to share ancestors
(Donnelly and Tavare 1995: 410), and thus lead to an accelerated time of origin for American
Indians and thus not truthfully demonstrating the occupational time depth American Indians have
in the Americas.
Furthermore, departures from random mating due to inbreeding, assortative mating, or
population stratification can lead to non-random association between genotypes and further
complicate the interpretation of the data and coalescent times. One such example is the well-
documented moiety and clan system among the Tlingit peoples of southern Alaska. Among the

Page 7
6
Tlingit, marriage was always with a member of one’s opposite moiety, and preferably with a
member of the father’s clan and house (De Laguna 1975). Therefore, the Tlingit as well as many
other American Indian groups of the Northwest Coast and other regions practiced a highly
selective, non-random form of mating that could influence the genetic data (see Handbook of
North American Indian series published by the Smithsonian Institution). There is also a growing
body of evidence suggesting that there could have been various forms of admixture between
American Indians, Japanese, and Russians during the last 500 years (Quimby 1985; van Stone
1984; Boyd 1999), not to mention known examples of admixture during the historic period with
trappers, fur traders, explorers, and other Europeans. Likewise, Karafet et al. (1997) concluded
that because of the presence of the 1T haplotype (a Y chromosome combination haplotype [see
next section for a discussion of haplotypes]) in both northeastern Siberia and the Americas, the
possibility of historic and prehistoric back-migration is extremely likely. Similar studies have
also noted the possibility of gene transfer or the “hitch-hiking theory” among American Indian
and Asian populations (Bianchi et al. 1997; Bradman and Thomas 1998; Hudson 1990). Because
population-coalescence times are frequently a result of the fusion of several of the ancient
phylogenetic clusters and not the age of individual populations (Watson et al. 1997), faulty
results may be reported. It is evident that neither American Indians nor specific American Indian
groups were ever isolated populations and that the history of a contemporary group’s genes are
not a specific history of that American Indian population. Therefore, using gene coalescent
times as possible times of origins for American Indians can lead to spurious conclusions, for
there is no evidence that American Indians were ever: 1) part of a neutral system that can be
timed like a regularly clicking clock, 2) were isolated from each other or from Asian populations,
and 3) that the current genes systems found in a particular population fully represent the diversity

Page 8
7
and history of that population.
II. Current Uses of Haplogroups
Although it has been noted that limitations exist when studying only one gene (Chen et
al. 2000; Karafet et al. 1997; Mountain and Cavalli-Sforza 1997), most studies still rely on only
one gene and its alleles because of the ease in identifying differences in a restricted location on
that gene, especially in non-recombining genes such as mtDNA. The allele sequences that are
studied are called haplotypes, which for American Indians presently fall into five recognized
haplogroups (A, B, C, D, and X), and have been used in most studies concerning American
Indian population genetics.
One of the current limitations with the uses of haplogroups for inferring American Indian
cultural affiliation is that there is the possibility of discovering new haplotypes as more tribes are
studied and techniques develop (Easton et al. 1996; Karafet et al. 1997; Schurr et al. 1990; Smith
et al. 1999). By testing only for known haplotype frequencies, it is likely that other haplotypes
will go undetected, resulting in spurious conclusions from simplified haplotype frequencies.
Along with the possibility of new haplotypes being discovered, it is known that many prehistoric
American Indian groups were not stationary and the use of within-local-population frequencies
for the genetic sequences, highly affected by each population’s specific recent demographic
history, and thus when relying on the use of haplogroups, scientists will probably underestimate
the nucleotide diversity of American Indians as a whole (Bonatto and Salzano 1997). Therefore,
the differing results between CR (control region) sequences and RFLP (restriction fragment
length polymorphism) data cannot be explained either by sample size or attributed to the
different ways in which the haplotype frequencies were treated, but are more probably due to the

Page 9
8
different populations or regions of the mtDNA studied. Furthermore, the only changes
introduced in genes are point mutations, insertions, and deletions (with insertions and deletions
being rare in comparison to point mutations). This means that each of the four possible founding
lineage clusters can be thought of as containing the founding lineage haplotype plus a collection
of that lineage’s descendants. However, as has been noted for the Y chromosome, the original Y
chromosome can eventually die out, shifting time, haplotype frequency, or relationships
(Bradman and Thomas 1998) and can result in faulty data when comparing present American
Indian tribal frequencies to those of ancient American Indian haplotype frequencies. As
Bradman and Thomas (1998) pointed out using the insertion of the YAP (Y chromosome alu
polymorphism) indel (insert) on the Y chromosome, descendents of individuals after only one
generation may not carry the same Y chromosome alleles. It is possible that a descendent of the
individual who first acquired the YAP indel may lose that indel, yet still remain a descendent of
that individual. This is also possible with mtDNA, where a father’s son or daughter will not
carry the genetic information of that person’s father’s mother. By only looking at specific
alleles, mutations, insertions, and deletions can be viewed as coming from discontinuous
populations. Likewise, “the combination of a decrease in the effective population size and
genetic hitch-hiking may have been the cause producing a single variety of Y-chromosomes in
the earliest ancestors of extant Amerindians,” (Bianchi et al. 1997: 87) which would result in
faulty results in determining affiliation between American Indian groups. Similarly, because the
mitochondrial genome undergoes no recombination, the 16,569-bp genome behaves
evolutionarily as a single locus. As MacEachern (2000: 358) recently notes, “In particular, it
appears that there may be significant variability in selection mechanisms on the genome itself
and in the mitochondria and in rates of phylogenetic versus intergenerational mtDNA mutation

Page 10
9
that are only now being appreciated (Gibbons 1998; Parsons, Muniec, and Sullivan 1997).”
Therefore, inferences from any one such locus lack robustness (Pamilo and Nei 1988). As noted
above, because of the potential inaccuracy in using a constant molecular clock, estimates of
mutation rates are going to be imprecise (Donnelly and Tavare 1995; Hoelzer et al. 1998).
Because of the high mobility of American Indian groups in the prehistoric, along with examples
of intergroup marriage and non-random mating, there is ample reason to believe that the genetic
history of American Indians is much more complex then the current five haplogroup frequencies
lead us to believe.
III. Sample Size
Many of the discrepancies and much of the unreliability of the data employed in
American Indian genetic studies lies in the sample sizes of the populations used. Variations in
population size are commonly attributed to bottlenecks and the so-called founder principle in
which a population encounters a severe reduction in size or a few individuals colonize a new area
resulting in a small selection of gene frequencies as compared with the original population.
However, an important complication that makes it impossible to determine census size of a
prehistoric human group as a direct estimate of the effective population size is that human
populations have overlapping generations. Rogers and Jorde (1995: 1-36) have shown that the
only sense in which sequence diversity can be employed as a measure of age is as an estimation
of the time during which a particular population has expanded after experiencing a severe
bottleneck. This is because we are dealing with alleles (haplotypes), and not with distinct
populations. In fact, the error variance increases with time and the earliest observations are the
most precise. Computer simulations that suggest that the four major haplogroups found among

Page 11
10
American Indians underwent a bottleneck followed by a large population expansion may be
questioned. These simulations are based primarily on the analysis of CR sequences from
haplogroup A and do not take into account haplogroups B, C, D, and X (as well as the possibility
of future haplogroups being discovered). Similarly, although most studies on the problem of
dating the original occupation of the Americas have used sequence diversity as a measure of age,
few have investigated whether their samples met the very stringent assumptions required by this
practice (Bonatto and Salzano 1997: 1417). Furthermore, Bonatto and Salzano (1997: 1417)
have also noted that studies using RFLPs found that haplogroup B had a much lower diversity
than the other three (A, C, D) which would lead to inaccurate computer simulations. Based on
this, the current dates from mtDNA and Y chromosome studies contending that American
Indians arrived in the “New World” around 35,000 years ago can be questioned (Bonatto and
Salzano 1997; Brown et al. 1998). This number is actually the time during which American
Indians theoretically experienced an expansion after a bottleneck. However, it is unknown if this
bottleneck took place in Asia, the generally accepted origin of American Indians, or in the
Americas after their arrival, nor is it known what effects migrations and subsequent bottlenecks
from disease and other factors have on this time estimation. Therefore, the date of 35,000 years
ago could be the time one group of American Indians entered the “New World” or when a group
experienced a bottleneck in Asia and subsequently entered the Americas, or any number of other
possible scenarios.
Another problem with the current sample sizes being used is the actual numbers of
individuals tested to infer the genetic makeup of the entire population. Typically, sample sizes
range between four and 30 individuals per tribal population; this is insufficient to detect little
more than the most common haplotypes in each population. Although it is necessary to have

Page 12
11
genetic samples from 50 males or 50 females of an individual population to accurately infer
genetic demographic history, no study has done this (Wells 2000). The largest study to date on
American Indians dealt with 2,198 males from 60 global populations, including 20 American
Indian groups (Karafet et al. 1999; this study relied on large amounts of data gathered from
previously published reports, and thus could not correct for those sample sizes). However, only
the Inuit Eskimo and Navajo samples were over 50 at 62 males and 56 males respectively. All
others ranged from as high as 44 to as low as two individuals. It is unrealistic to assume that one
can get an accurate picture of a tribe’s genetic frequencies using only two males. In fact, Weiss
(1994: 834) suggests that we may not be able to distinguish loss of lineages after one migration
or from separate migrations from a common source population, thus further stressing the critical
need for adequate population sample sizes. A clear example of the importance of sample size is
seen in Easton et al.’s (1996) study and Torroni et al.’s (1993) study on the Yanomamo. In
Easton et al.’s sample they detected both haplotypes X6 and X7, but in Torroni et al.’s sample
from a neighboring village they did not detect any of these two haplotypes. As Ward et al.
(1993) have noted, a sample size of 25 will detect ~63 percent of the lineages in a tribe with
normal diversity. In tribes with extensive diversity a sample size of 25 individuals will only
detect ~40 percent of the lineages and sample sizes of 70 or above are required to detect two-
thirds of the lineages. The fact that the majority of studies lack the required sample sizes
necessary to detect even 63 percent of the lineages in a normally diverse tribe brings into
question many of the results of these studies, especially when it has been noted that most
American Indian tribes are believed to have a high level of diversity (Ward et al. 1993).
In the past, as now, choice of mates is largely dictated by geographic, socioeconomic,
religious, ethnic, and other constraints. This has the effect of subdividing and stratifying the

Page 13
12
gene pool of a population in very complex ways. Likewise, migration is also difficult to
reconstruct from mtDNA and Y chromosomes. The most meaningful measure of migration from
a genetic point of view is obtained by taking the generation as the time unit. Measuring the
distribution between birthplaces of parent and offspring theoretically can yield a statistical
measure of migration. However, this method works only for a continuous model in which the
population is constant, and is not entirely satisfactory when the population is highly clustered as
is believed most prehistoric American Indian populations were (Cavalli-Sforza and Bodmer
1971:433). A similar limitation in using such data to infer migrations is that exchange between
non-neighboring clusters is frequent enough among American Indians to violate the rules of the
simplest stepping-stone models (Cavalli-Sforza and Bodmer 1971:433).
Another aspect of human DNA confounding many of the current uses of this data to
reconstruct hypothetical demographic histories is that human mtDNA variation is high.
Likewise, genetic variation within populations is much greater than between populations
(Walpoff 1999:551). What this means is that mtDNA evolution, and possibly the evolution of
other genetic systems, is not the same as the evolution of particular populations. As Scozzori et
al. (1999) have noted, groups or tribes thought to have descended from a common ancestor more
than 10,000 years ago may have lost even their shared-by-descent portion of their gene pool and
can no longer be detected as affiliated through genetic analysis. Likewise, population specific
mutations and the gene trees inferred from these sequences are generally inconsistent with
historic and prehistoric population affiliation. Page and Charleston (1990) have identified a
method for visualizing and quantifying the relationship between a pair of gene and species trees
that constructs a third, reconciled tree. Reconciled trees use a more critically optimal method for
mapping the combined history of genes and populations. However, even this more accurate

Page 14
13
method of depicting gene and population trees has limitations such as allele phylogenies and
horizontal transfer, neither of which has been addressed in studies concerning American Indian
demographic history. In fact, many of the polymorphisms observed for mtDNA probably
predates population separations (Mountain and Cavalli-Sforza 1997) and would not be useful in
constructing genetic, population, or reconciled trees. Mitochondrial DNA or Y chromosome
lineages are not human populations. In order to estimate the significance of variation of gene
frequencies between groups, it is necessary to estimate how large a sample must be in order to be
representative of the group. This can only be accomplished if an accurate estimate of the real
variation to be expected in the gene frequencies is possible. This estimation is valid only for
genes without dominance, in which case genes can be counted. However, if people in the sample
from a given tribal village or town are closely related, a single source of variation may greatly
inflate the estimate of variance between populations (Cavalli-Sforza and Bodmer 1971:422).
Multivariate analysis, or the use of more than one trait or gene, which is presently the most
commonly employed method of analysis, poses more difficult problems in that one must
determine the maximum number of genes possible for each population in order to be accurate.
Unfortunately, many authors have tested only a small set of markers on one gene (univariate) for
their studies (Cavalli-Sforza et al. 1994: 22), combining their data with those of others to result
in several sets of markers to arrive at their multivariate analysis. Not only have limited numbers
of markers been studied and subsequently combined with other studies (which was noted above),
but the mutation rate for insertions and deletions on those markers is unknown.

Page 15
14
IV. Language
Not only have sample sizes of groups or tribes being tested been inadequate, but most
studies have relied on the use of controversial linguistic phyla in order to place their data into
objective, quantifiable groups. However, as several papers have pointed out, not only do the
correspondences between languages and populations differ (i.e., Barbujani 1997; Karafet et al.
1999; Scozzari et al. 1999; Schurr et al. 1999), but there is no agreed upon set of linguistic phyla
for American Indians (Greenberg et al. 1986; Greenberg 1987; Bateman et al. 1990; O’Grady et
al. 1989; Ruhlen 1987, 1994). Most studies use several linguistic phyla that are subject to
serious criticism, such as Altaic s.l., Austric s.l., Indo-Pacific, Amerind (sensu Greenberg 1987),
and Na-dene s.l., which are in turn awarded equal status as more accepted phyla from other parts
of the world such as Sino-Tibetan, Indo-European, and Dravidian (Bateman et al. 1990).
Furthermore, it has been noted that “given 56% correspondence between linguistic phyla and
population aggregates at the coarse level of resolution, 11% correspondence at the fine level, and
the poor integrity of both superphyla, the parallelism between the genetic and linguistic entities
does not strike us as especially ‘remarkable’” (Bateman et al. 1990:7). Likewise, “there is an
important problem of time scales involved in this work, since at this point neither genetic nor
linguistic research can lay claim to chronometric techniques comparable in precision to those
used by archaeologists and historians” (Pluciennik 1995:44-45). Languages do not change at
specific rates and therefore using contemporary linguistic phyla to extrapolate prehistoric
population groups is ill-founded. For example, in 1995 there were approximately 209 native
North American languages still spoken, close to only half the number that existed five hundred

Page 16
15
years earlier (Goddard 1996). Similarly, of the Eastern Algonquian languages, only seven were
spoken in 1970 out of a total of 20 from 200 years earlier (Goddard 1978).
Many of the researchers conducting genetic tests have noted the discrepancies between
linguistic phyla and genetic phyla. Scozzari et al. (1999) concluded that geography is a better
method for identifying affiliation then linguistics. Likewise, Poloni et al. (1997) concluded that
genetic data is more accurate and useful for distinguishing between linguistic phyla than between
populations in the same language family. Finally, Schurr et al. (1999) noted that populations on
the Kamchatka peninsula were genetically similar based on geography but quite divergent when
compared to linguistically related groups. Therefore, the use of linguistic phyla may be useful
when studying the differences between language phylas (i.e., between Na-Dene and Eskimo-
Aluet), but not as useful when studying groups within the same language phyla (i.e., Yakama and
Nez Perce). Not taking into account the current discrepancies between American Indian
language phylas can lead to several different conclusions depending on how the linguistic and
genetic data are combined. For example, Karafet et al. (1997) found that Y chromosome
markers did not agree with the linguistic phyla proposed by Greenberg et al. (1986) for the
peopling of the Americas. However, in a later study using different Y chromosome markers
Karafet et al. (1999) did agree with the linguistic phyla proposed by Greenberg et al. (1986).
Other studies have arrived at similarly contradictory conclusions (see Schurr et al. 1999; Poloni
et al. 1997).
To use current American Indian languages as a baseline for prehistoric American
Indian genetic affiliations and population groups seems presumptuous. Until linguistic
specialists agree upon the classifications of American Indian languages, they should not be used
as a means of inferring and objectifying prehistoric population groups.

Page 17
16
V. Contemporary American Indian Reservations and Demographic History
As noted above, the current sample sizes of most studies fall far short of a reasonable
number of individuals being tested to be considered an accurate data set of the population.
However, besides the limitations arising from the small sample sizes, as well as those discussed
concerning the present use of linguistic phyla, there are even greater problems lying in what the
studies consider populations. Presently, studies concerning American Indian cultural affiliation
and demographic history test individuals from a reservation and combine their allele frequencies
to arrive at the haplotype makeup of that population. Therefore, the researchers are using
contemporary American Indian reservation demographics to arrive at a population that they then
infer back into prehistory. However, one of the primary problems with this method is that most
contemporary American Indian reservations are not made up of a single group, but consist of
several different groups of American Indians that prior to being forced onto reservations were
autonomous groups. For example, Merriwether et al. (1995) use samples from Haida, Dogrib,
and other contemporary American Indian reservation groups which they consider as one
population group. However, the Dogrib as a whole tribe were prehistorically made up of several
different bands that occupied a large area in the Northwest Territories, Canada, between the
Great Slave Lake in the south to the Great Bear Lake in the north and from the lowlands on the
east side of the Mackenzie River to Contwoyto, Aylmer, and Artillery Lakes. The Dogrib are
known to have had regular contact with the Bearlake Indians, the Slaveys, Chipewyans, and
occasionally Eskimos (Helm 1981: 291). Likewise, the Haida, along with other Northwest Coast
tribes were known to have traded slaves up and down the coast. The Haida traded slaves they
acquired from the Kwakiutl with the Tlingit (Blackman 1990). Other such examples can be
found in the studies by Smith et al. (1999), Karafet et al. (1999), Lorenz et al. (1996), and Brown

Page 18
17
et al. (1998) that use contemporary reservation groups as prehistoric population groups. Such
contemporary groups as the Yakama and Apache are good examples. The present Yakama
reservation in Washington is made up of at least five different groups that were prehistorically
independent bands or groups (Schuster 1998). Similarly, there is still much disagreement among
American Indian specialists as to how many different Apache groups there were prior to the
arrival of Euroamericans. Currently there are seven recognized Southern Apachean speaking
groups: Chiricahua, Jicarilla, Kiowa-Apache, Lipan, Mescalero, Navajo, and Western Apache.
However, depending on “how much more extensive their territories are conceived to have been
in the past depends upon one’s view of claims that the Querechos, Vaqueros, Teyas, Janos,
Jocomes, Mansos, Sumas, Cholomes, Jumanos, Cibolos, Pelones, Padoucas, and various other
groups named in early Spanish and French records were Apacheans” (Opler 1983:368). Finally,
over the last hundred years reservation populations have been greatly affected by outmarriage
with other tribal groups and marriage with non-Indians. An example of this change can be seen
in a study done by Walker (1990; see also Walker 1972) for the Confederated Tribes of the
Umatilla Indian Reservation (CTUIR). This study showed that in 1990 54 different tribes were
represented in the blood of CTUIR individuals (see Table 1). Furthermore, one CTUIR
individual had various amounts of Cayuse, Walla Walla, Umatilla, Nez Perce, Snohomis, and
non-Indian blood, while another individual had Umatilla, Cayuse, Walla Walla, Yakama, Nez
Perce, Quinault, Snoqualmie, Cascade, and non-Indian blood. It is evident that the population
groups current studies are using to infer American Indian cultural affiliation and demographic
history are not acceptable. One cannot use contemporary allele frequencies from a few
individuals of a contemporary American Indian reservation to arrive at an unequivocal haplotype
for that group, either presently or prehistorically.

Page 19
18
Table 1: Blood Types found in CTUIR Tribal Members
Tribal Affiliation
Geographic Proximity
Geographic Location
Alaskan
Distant
Arctic
Arikara
Distant
Plains
Assiniboin
Distant
Plains
Bannock
Distant
Great Basin
Blackfoot
Distant
Plains
Canadian
Distant
Cascade
Neighboring
Plateau
Cayuse
Official Blood Line
Plateau
Cherokee
Distant
Plains
Cheyenne
Distant
Plains
Chippewa
Distant
Plains/Subarctic
Chocktaw
Distant
Southeast
Cochiti
Distant
Southwest
Coeur d’Alene
Neighboring
Plateau
Colville
Neighboring
Plateau
Cowichen
Distant
Coast
Cowlitz
Neighboring
Plateau
Cree
Distant
Plains/Subarctic
Crow
Distant
Plains
Flathead
Neighboring
Plateau
Grande Ronde
Distant
Coast
Hopi
Distant
Southwest
Klamath
Neighboring
Plateau
Klickitat
Neighboring
Plateau
Kootenai
Neighboring
Plateau
Laguna
Distant
Southwest
Lummi
Distant
Coast
Makah
Distant
Coast
Modoc
Distant
Coast
Muckleshoot
Distant
Coast
Navajo
Distant
Southwest

Page 20
19
Nez Perce
Neighboring
Plateau
Ottawa
Distant
Northeast
Paiute
Distant
Great Basin
Palus
Neighboring
Plateau
Pawnee
Distant
Plains
Puyallup
Distant
Coast
Quinault
Distant
Coast
Sac and Fox
Distant
Plains
Seminole
Distant
Southeast
Shoshone
Distant
Great Basin
Siletz
Distant
Coast
Sioux
Distant
Plains
Snohomish
Distant
Coast
Spokane
Neighboring
Plateau
Tulalip
Distant
Coast
Walla Walla
Official Blood Line
Plateau
Warm Springs
Neighboring
Plateau
Wasco
Neighboring
Plateau
White Mountain Apache
Distant
Southwest
Winnebago
Distant
Plains
Wishram
Neighboring
Plateau
Yakama
Neighboring
Plateau
A further problem in the use of contemporary American Indian reservations can be found
in the use of ancient DNA (aDNA). Several reports have used aDNA to construct ancient
populations that are then compared to present American Indian reservation populations. These
studies are plagued by many of the limitations already noted such as sample size, demographic
histories, the possibility of mutation addition or deletion, and other factors. Kaestle (1997)
attempted to compare an ancient population from western Nevada to those of contemporary
tribal populations in the region through haplogroup frequencies. However, Kaestle’s ancient

Page 21
20
population spanned 5000 years in time. A genetic sample dating to 5905+/-125 years BP cannot
be considered part of the same population as a genetic sample dating to 860+/-75 years BP
without also automatically designating contemporary American Indians as part of that
population. “Ancient DNA samples are not populations in the traditional sense of the term. The
individual specimens that constitute aDNA samples may span several centuries and even
geographic space. Thus they are the equivalent of sampling an individual every few generations
to characterize a continuous population” (O’Rourke et al. 2000). This is especially true when
these samples are then compared to contemporary American Indian reservations that each have
their own, unique demographic history. Likewise, the use of aDNA models to reconstruct a
population assume (or cannot accurately model) that these ancient populations were somewhat
isolated, and that these populations did not practice forms of intergroup or outgroup marriage.
However, as previously noted for the Northwest Coast and the Plateau regions, highly complex
forms of intergroup marriage have been practiced for centuries. Complex forms of intergroup
marriage have also been documented for Great Basin tribes (see D’Azevedo 1986). Not only has
there been extensive intergroup marriage within regions, but also between regions as noted in
Walker (1972, 1990).
Finally, it should be noted that many of the scientists conducting these studies acquire a
large proportion of their blood samples or genetic material through convenience samples.
Convenience samples means that the blood samples or genetic material were not collected by the
investigating scientist, but instead through third parties. Many of these third parties initially
acquired the blood or genetic material for other reasons, such as diabetes testing. A review of
the literature has revealed that over a hundred institutions have allowed these scientists access to
American Indian blood, a lot of the time without the individual who gave the blood having any

Page 22
21
knowledge of this. Though there are many problems with this in and of itself, the point that is
important in the present discussion is that the scientists have no means of verifying the actual
tribal affiliation of the blood sample they are using. For example, when an individual goes in for
diabetes testing, they designate themselves and their tribal affiliation, through there is no
guarantee that this designation is correct, nor is there any knowledge of that individuals family
genetic history. This fact could greatly mislead the scientists into concluding various tribal
haplotype frequencies that may not be correct.
VI. Genetics and American Indian Cultural Affiliation
The fact that most studies have not addressed the above concerns is only part of the
present problem with applying anthropological genetics to American Indian demographic history
and cultural affiliation. In fact, no studies concerning American Indians have seriously taken
into account the demographic history of the last 500 years when Euroamericans arrived in the
“New World.” Almost every contemporary American Indian tribe or group in the Americas has
experienced severe epidemic diseases, depopulation, acculturation, and displacement from their
native lands. These factors have caused some tribes to disappear, others have experienced
population fluctuations greater than 80 percent, and some have been displaced from their home
land by hundreds of miles (Boyd 1999; Dobyns 1983; Ehle 1988; Jones 2002). For example, the
American Indians of California numbered upwards of 310,000 during the eighteenth century, but
by the turn of the twentieth century the native population had dropped to 20,000 (Cook 1978).
Similar population declines are known for the Plateau with a loss of approximately 20,000
Natives between 1805 and 1860 (Boyd 1990), the Northwest with a population decline from
approximately 200,000 Natives in 1774 to 40,000 in 1874 (Boyd 1990, 2000), as well as other

Page 23
22
regions. Furthermore, admixture with historic Europeans and Euroamericans such as fur
trappers, explorers, African Americans, and earlier settlers must be accounted for; it is well
known, though not well documented, that many of these non-indigenous peoples married or
mixed with American Indians.
Any mtDNA or Y chromosome study that attempts to date the first appearance of a
particular population in a certain geographical area as big as the Americas or as small as the
Great Basin should be based on extensive sampling, not only of the population under
consideration but also of potential source populations and neighboring populations. A
phylogenetic analysis can then theoretically identify the putative founder sequences. This
requires that the mutations distinguishing these founders must be disregarded in order to set the
evolutionary clock to zero to coincide with the arrival of a particular population in the new area.
Considerable scientific care must be exercised in choosing realistic demographic models to
describe the process adequately. For example, current models that assume constant population
size and random mating would be unrealistic in most situations for the following reasons: 1)
arrival in a new area is likely to trigger subsequent population expansion for many generations;
2) for a population on the move, the demographic pressure is more relaxed, allowing population
sizes to fluctuate rather freely, without statistically conforming to an expected (time-dependent)
size; 3) over a period of tens of thousands of years, environmental conditions often change
drastically, obviously influencing the effective population sizes; and 4) ongoing gene flow from
neighboring populations will inevitably distort the estimation of nucleotide diversity, and thus
the coalescence time estimate (Forster et al., 1996: 944).

Page 24
23
Conclusion
Although mtDNA and Y chromosome studies can provide insights on America Indian
origins and prehistoric relationships, they should be used with caution. Mitochondrial DNA and
Y chromosome studies are in their infancy. Because of the various limitations listed above, as
well as a lack of correlation between anthropological genetic data, archaeological data,
ethnographic data, and oral tradition, these studies should be viewed as inchoate and requiring
further investigation and support from the other fields of anthropology. Current controversies
surrounding Spirit Cave Man and Kennewick Man should not be resolved only by mtDNA or Y
chromosome testing. The mtDNA and Y chromosome data for American Indians, as well as
many other regions throughout the world, have serious limitations. However, because of the
claimed authoritative validity of these studies there is great danger that they will convince non-
specialists of the validity of the hypothesized associations between American Indian groups. The
non-specialists would be ill-advised to rely on the claims put forth by mtDNA and Y
chromosome inferred American Indian studies because they take little account of the vast
majority of ethnographic, linguistic, historical, and archaeological research. However,
archaeologists, ethnologists, linguists, and historians should take note of some of the inferences
currently being made by anthropological geneticists. It is believed that mtDNA and Y
chromosome data offer an opportunity for discourse among our often disparate fields, allowing
us to achieve a greater understanding of American Indian cultural affiliation and demographic
history. However, further studies should gain tribal approval before using tribal blood,
especially when claims are subsequently made denying various affiliations between that tribe’s
blood and other groups or ancient skeletons.

Page 25
24
References Cited
Bailliet, G., F. Rothhammer, F.R. Carnese, C.M. Bravi, and N.O. Bianchi. 1994. Founder
Mitochondrial Haplotypes in Amerindian Populations. American Journal of Human Genetics,
54:27-33.
Barbujani, Guido. 1997. DNA Variation and Language Affinities. American Journal of Human
Genetics, 61:1011-1014.
Bateman, Richard, Ives Goddard, Richard O’Grady, V.A. Funk, Rich Mooi, W. John Kress, and
Peter Cannell. 1990. Speaking of Forked Tongues: The Feasibility of Reconciling Human
Phylogeny and the History of Language. Current Anthropology, 31(1):1-13.
Bradman, Neil and Mark Thomas. 1998. Why Y? The Y Chromosome in the Study of Human
Evolution, Migration, and Prehistory. Science Spectra, 14:32-37.
Bianchi, N.O., G. Bailliet, C.M. Bravi. 1994. Peopling of the Americas as Inferred Through The
Analysis of Mitochondrial DNA. Brazil Journal of Genetics, 18:661-668.
Bianchi, Nestor, Graciela Bailliet, Claudio Bravi, Raul Carnese, Francisco Rothhammer,
Veronica Martinez-Marignac, and Sergio Pena. 1997. Origin of Amerindian Y-Chromosomes as
Inferred by the Analysis of Six Polymorphic Markers. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 102:79-89.
Blackman, Margaret B. 1990. Haida: Traditional Culture. In Handbook of North American
Indians, Vol. 7: Northwest Coast. Edited by Wayne Suttles. Washington D.C.: Government
Printing Office.
Bonatto, Sandro L. and Francisco M. Salzano. 1997. Diversity and Age of the Four Major
mtDNA Haplogroups, and Their Implications for the Peopling of the New World. American
Journal of Human Genetics, 61:1413-1423.
Boyd, Robert. 1990. Demographic History, 1774-1874. In Handbook of North American Indians,
Vol. 7: Northwest Coast. Wayne Suttles ed. Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office.
Boyd, Robert. 1999. The Coming of the Spirit of Pestilence: Introduced Infectious Diseases and
Population Decline among Northwest Coast Indians, 1774-1874. Seattle: University of
Washington Press.

Page 26
25
Brown, Michael D., Seyed H. Hosseini, Antonio Torroni, Hans-Jurgen Bandelt, Jon C. Allen,
Theodore G. Schurr, Rosaria Scozzari, Fulvio Cruciani, and Douglas C. Wallace. 1998. mtDNA
Haplogroup X: An Ancient Link between Europe/Western Asia and North America? American
Journal of Human Genetics, 63:1852-1861.
Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., and W.F. Bodmer. 1971. The Genetics of Human Populations. San
Francisco: W.H. Freeman and Company.
Cavalli-Sforza, L. Luca, Paolo Menozzi, and Alberto Piazza. 1994. The History and Geography
of Human Genetics. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press.
Chatters, James C. 2000. The Recovery and First Analysis of an Early Holocene Human
Skeleton from Kennewick, Washington. American Antiquity, 65(2):291-316.
Chen, Yu-Sheng, Antonel Olckers, Theodore G. Schurr, Andreas M. Kogelnik, Kirsi Huoponen,
and Douglas C. Wallace. 2000. mtDNA Variation in the South African Kung and Khwe- and
Their Genetic Relationship to Other African Populations. American Journal of Human Genetics,
66:1362-1383.
Cook, Sherburne. 1978. Historical Demography. In Handbook of North American Indians, Vol.
8: California. Robert F. Heizer ed. Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office.
D’Azevedo, Warren L. 1986. Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 11: Great Basin.
Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office.
De Laguna, Frederica. 1975. Matrilineal Kin Groups in Northwestern North America. In Volume
1 of Proceedings: Northern Athapaskan Conference, 1971. A. McFadyen Clark, ed. Canada.
National Museum of Man. Mercury Series. Ethnology Service Papers 27. Ottawa.
Dillehay, Tom D. 2000 The Settlement of the Americas: A New Prehistory. Basic Books: New
York.
Dobyns, Henry. 1983. Their Number Became Thinned: Native American Population Dynamics in
Eastern North America. Knoxville: University of Tennessee Press.
Donnelly, Peter, and Simon Tavare. 1995. Coalescents and Genealogical Structure Under
Neutrality. Annual Review of Genetics, 29:401-421.
Easton, Ruth D., D. Andrew Merriwether, Douglas E. Crews, and Robert E. Ferrell. 1996.
mtDNA Variation in the Yanomami: Evidence for Additional New World Founding Lineages.
American Journal of Human Genetics, 59:213-225.
Ehle, John. 1988. Trail of Tears: The Rise and Fall of the Cherokee Nation. New York: Anchor
Books.

Page 27
26
Flannery, Kent V., and Joyce Marcus. 1994. Early Formative Pottery of the Valley of Oaxaca,
Mexico. Memoirs of the Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan, No. 27.
Forster, Peter, Rosalind Harding, Antonio Torroni, and Hans-Jurgen Bandelt. 1996. Origin and
Evolution of Native American mtDNA Variation: A Reappraisal. American Journal of Human
Genetics, 59:935-945.
Gibbons, A. 1998. Calibrating the molecular clock. Science, 279:28-29.
Goddard, Ives. 1978. A Further Note on Pidgin English. International Journal of American
Linguistics, 44(1):73.
Goddard, Ives. 1996. Introduction. In Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 17: Languages.
Ives Goddard ed. Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office.
Greenberg, Joseph H. 1987. Language in the Americas. Stanford University Press, California.
Greenberg, J.H., C.G. Turner II, and S.L. Zegura. 1986. The Settlement of the Americas: A
Comparison of the Linguistic, Dental, and Genetic Evidence. Current Anthropology, 27:477-497.
Hayden, Brian editor. 1992. A Complex Culture of the British Columbia Plateau: Traditional
Stl’atl’imx Resource Use. Vancouver: UBC Press.
Helm, June. 1981. Dogrib. In Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 6: Subarctic. June
Helm editor, Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office. Pp. 291-309.
Hoelzer, Guy A., Joel Wallman, Don J. Melnick. 1998. The Effects of Social Structure,
Geographical Structure, and Population Size on the Evolution of Mitochondrial DNA: II.
Molecular Clocks and the Lineage Sorting Period. Journal of Molecular Evolution, 48:21-31.
Hudson, Richard R. 1990. Gene Genealogies and the Coalescent Process. Oxford Surveys in
Evolutionary Biology, 7:1-44.
Jones, Peter. 2002. Old World Infectious Diseases in the Plateau Area of North America during
the Protohistoric: Rethinking Our Understanding of the “Contact” in the Plateau. In circulation.
Kaestle, Frederika. 1997. Molecular Analysis of Ancient Native American DNA From Western
Nevada. Nevada Historical Society Quarterly, 40(1):85-96.
Kaestle, Frederika, and David Glenn Smith. 2001. American Journal of Physical Anthoropology,
115:1-12.

Page 28
27
Karafet, Tatiana, Stephen L. Zegura, Jennifer Vuturo-Brady, Olga Posukh, Ludmila Osipova,
Victor Wiebe, Francine Romero, Jeffery C. Long, Shinji Harihara, Feng Jin, Bumbein
Dashnyam, Tudevdagva Gerelsaikhan, Keiichi Omoto, and Michael F. Hammer. 1997. Y
Chromosome Markers and Trans-Bering Strait Dispersals. American Journal of Physical
Anthropology, 102:301-314.
Karafet, T.M., S.L. Zegura, O. Posukh, L. Osipova, A. Bergen, J. Long, D. Goldman, W. Klitz,
S. Harihara, P. de Knijff, V. Wiebe, R.C. Griffiths, A.R. Templeton, and M.F. Hammer. 1999.
Ancestral Asian Source(s) of New World Y-Chromosome Founder Haplotypes. American
Journal of Human Genetics, 64:817-831.
Lekson, Stephen H. 2000. The Chaco Meridian: Centers of Political Power in the Ancient
Southwest. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Lewontin, R.C. 1971. The Apportionment of Human Diversity. In Evolutionary Biology, Vol. 6.
T.H. Dobzhansky et al., eds. Pp. 381-398. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Lorenz, Joseph G., and David Glenn Smith. 1996. Distribution of Four Founding mtDNA
Haplogroups Among Native North Americans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
101:307-323.
MacEachern, Scott. 2000. Genes, Tribes, and African History. Current Anthropology, 41(3)357-
384.
Matson, G.A. 1938. Blood groups and ageusia in Indians of Montana and Alberta. American
Journal of Physical Anthropology, 24:81-89.
Matson, G.A., and H.F. Schrader. 1933. Blood grouping among the “Blackfeet” and “Blood”
tribes of American Indians. Journal of Immunology 25:15-163.
Merriwether, D. Andrew, Francisco Rothhammer, and Robert E. Ferrell. 1995. Distribution of
the Four Founding Lineage Haplotypes in Native Americans Suggests a Single Wave of
Migration for the New World. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 98:411-430.
Mountain, Joanna L., and L. Luca Cavalli-Sforza. 1997. Multilocus Genotypes, a Tree of
Individuals, and Human Evolutionary History. American Journal of Human Genetics, 61:705-
718.
Napton, L. Kyle. 1997. The Spirit Cave Mummy: Coprolite Investigations. Nevada Historical
Society Quarterly, 40(1):97-103.
O’Grady, R.T., I. Goddard, R.M. Bateman, W.A. Di Michele, V.A. Funk, W.J. Kress, R. Mooi,
and P.F. Cannell. 1989. Genes and Tongues. Science, 243:1651.

Page 29
28
O’Rourke, D.H., M.G. Hayes, and S.W. Carlyle. 2000. Spatial and Temporal Stability of mtDNA
Haplogroup Frequencies in Native North America. In Human Biology, 72(1):15-34.
Opler, Morris E. 1983. The Apachean Culture Pattern and Its Origins. In Handbook of North
American Indians, Vol. 10: The Southwest. Edited by Alfonso Ortiz. Washington D.C.:
Government Printing Office.
Page, Roderic D.M., and Michael A. Charleston. 1990. Reconciled Trees and Incongruent Gene
and Species Trees. DIMACS Series in Discrete Mathematics and Theoretical Computer Science,
Vol. 00: 1-14.
Pamilo, P., M. Nei. 1998. Relationships Between Gene Trees and Species Trees. Molecular
Biological Evolution, 5(5):568-583.
Parsons, T., D. Muniec, and K. Sullivan. 1997. A high observed substitution rate in the human
mitochondrial control region. Nature Genetics, 15:363-68.
Poloni, E.S., O. Semino, G. Passarino, A.S. Santachiara-Benerecetti, I. Dupanloup, A. Langaney,
and L. Excoffier. 1997. Human Genetic Affinities for Y-Chromosome P49a,f/TaqI Haplotypes
Show Strong Correspondence with Linguistics. American Journal of Human Genetics, 61:1015-
1035.
Pluciennik, M. 1995. A perilous but necessary search: Archaeology and European Identities. In
Nationalism and archaeology: Scottish Archaeological Forum. Edited by J. Atkinson and J.
O’Sullivan, pp. 35-58. Glasgow: Cruithne Press.
Quimby, George I. 1985. Japanese Wrecks, Iron Tools, and Prehistoric Indians of the Northwest
Coast. Arctic Anthropology, 22(2):7-15.
Rogers, A., and L. Jorde. 1995. Genetic Evidence on Modern Human Origins. Human Biology,
67:1-36.
Ruhlen, M. 1986. A Guide to the World’s Languages. Vol 1. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
Ruhlen, M. 1994. Linguistic Evidence for the Peopling of the Americas. In Methods and Theory
for Investigating the Peopling of the Americas, Bonnichsen and Steele editors. Center for the
Study of the First Americans, Corvallis: Oregon. Pp. 177-188.
Santos, Fabricio R., Arpita Pandya, Chris Tyler-Smith, Sergio D.J. Pena, Moses Schanfield,
William R. Leonard, Ludmila Osipova, Michael H. Crawford, and R. John Mitchell. 1999. The
Central Siberian Origin for Native American Y Chromosomes. American Journal of Human
Genetics, 64:619-628.

Page 30
29
Schurr, Theodore G., Scott W. Ballinger, Yik-Yuen Gan, Judith A. Hodge, D. Andrew
Merriwether, Dale N. Lawrence, William C. Knowler, Kenneth M. Weiss, and Douglas C.
Wallace. 1990. Amerindian Mitochondrial DNAs Have Rare Asian Mutations at High
Frequencies, Suggesting They Derived from Four Primary Maternal Lineages. American Journal
of Human Genetics, 46:613-623.
Schurr, Theodore G., Rem I. Sukernik, Yelena B. Starikovskaya, and Douglas C. Wallace. 1999.
Mitochondrial DNA Variation in Koryaks and Itel’men: Population Replacement in the Okhotsk
Sea- Bearing Sea Region During the Neolithic. American Journal of Physical Anthropology,
108:1-39.
Schuster, Helen. 1998. Yakima and Neighboring Groups. In Handbook of North American
Indians, Vol. 12: The Plateau. Deward E. Walker, Jr., ed. Washington D.C.: Government
Printing Office.
Scozzari, Rosaria, Fulvio Cruciani, Piero Santolamazza, Patrizia Malaspina, Antonio Torroni,
Daniele Sellitto, Barbara Arredi, Giovanni Destro-Bisol, Gianfranco De Stefano, Olga Rickards,
Cristina Martinez-Labarga, David Modiano, Gianfranco Biondi, Pedro Moral, Antonel Olckers,
Douglas C. Wallace, and Andrea Novelletto. 1999. Combined Use of Biallelic and Microsatellite
Y-Chromosome Polymorphisms to Infer Affinities among African Populations. American
Journal of Human Genetics, 65:829-846.
Smith, David Glenn, Ripan S. Malhi, Jason Eshleman, Joseph G. Lorenz, and Frederika A.
Kaestle. 1999. Distribution of mtDNA Haplogroup X Among Native North Americans.
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 110:271-284.
Stern, Theodore. 1998. Cayuse, Umatilla, and Walla Walla. In Handbook of North American
Indians, Vol. 12: The Plateau. Deward E. Walker, Jr., ed. Washington D.C.: Government
Printing Office.
Templeton, Alan R. 1993. The “Eve” Hypotheses: A Genetic Critique and Reanalysis. American
Anthropologist, 95(1):51-72.
Torroni, A., T.G. Schurr, C-C Yang, E.J.E. Szathmary, R.C. Williams, M.S. Shanfield, G.A.
Troup, et al. 1990. Native American Mitochondrial DNA Analysis Indicates that the Amerind
and Nadene Populations Were Founded by Two Independent Migrations. Genetics, 130:153-162.
Torroni, A., T.G. Schurr, M.F. Cabell, M.D. Brown, J.V. Neel, M. Larson, D.G. Smith, C.M.
Vullo, and D.C. Wallace. 1993. Asian Affinities and Continental Radiation of the Four Founding
Native American mtDNAs. American Journal of Human Genetics, 53:563-590.
VanStone, James W. 1984. Exploration and Contact History of Western Alaska. In Handbook of
North American Indians, Vol. 5: The Arctic. David Damas, ed. Washington D.C.: Government
Printing Office.

Page 31
30
Walker, Deward E., Jr. 1972. The Emergent Native Americans. Boston: Little, Brown and
Company.
Walker, Deward E., Jr. 1990. A Study of CRUIR Enrollment and Population. General Council
and Board of Trustees of the Confederated Tribes of the Umatilla Indian Reservation. Mission:
Oregon.
Walker, Deward E., Jr. 1998. Introduction. In Handbook of North American Indians, Vol. 12:
The Plateau. Deward E. Walker, Jr., ed. Washington D.C.: Government Printing Office.
Ward, R.H., Alan Redd, Diana Valencia, Barbara Frazier, and Svante Paabo. 1993. Genetic and
Linguistic Differentiation in the Americas. Proceedings of the National Acadamey of Sciences,
USA, 90:10663-10667.
Watson, Elizabeth, Peter Forster, Martin Richards, and Hans-Jurgen Bandelt. 1997.
Mitochondrial Footprints of Human Expansions in Africa. American Journal of Human
Genetics, 61:691-704.
Weiss, K.M. 1994. American Origins. Procession of the National Academy of Science, 91:833-
835.
Wells, Spencer. 2000. Cavalli-Sforza Laboratory, Stanford University Web Page.
Http://163.1.230.14/eurasia/htdocs/anthgen.html Accessed July 17, 2000.
Wolpoff, Milford. 1999. Paleoanthropology, 2
nd
Ed. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Boulder, Co. 80306