Child Neglect: Current Definitions and ModelsA review of child neglect research, 1993_1998 was prepared by Susan Sullivan for the Family Violence Prevention Unit, Health Canada.
Également en français sous le titre La négligence à l'égard des enfants : définitions et modèles actuelsExamen de la recherche portant sur la négligence à l'égard des enfants, 1993_1998.
The opinions expressed in this report are those of the author and
do not
necessarily reflect the views of Health Canada.
Contents may not be reproduced for commercial purposes, but any other reproduction, with acknowledgements, is encouraged.
This publication may be provided in alternate formats upon request.
For further information on family violence issues, please contact:
The National Clearinghouse on Family Violence
Family Violence Prevention Unit
Health Issues Division
Public Health Agency of Canada (PHAC)
Health Canada
Address Locator: 1909D1
9th Floor, Jeanne Mance Bldg., Tunney's Pasture
Ottawa, Ontario K1A 1B4 CANADA
Telephone: 1_800_267_1291 or (613) 957_2938
Fax: (613) 941_8930
Fax Link: 1_888_267_1233 or (613) 941_7285
TTY: 1_800_561_5643 or (613) 952_6396
Web Site: http://www.phac-aspc.gc.ca/nc-cn
© Her Majesty the Queen in Right of Canada, 2000
Cat. H72-21-171-2000E
ISBN 0-662-28466-6
A Brief History of Child Maltreatment Research 2
Defining Child Neglect 7
Severity of Consequences 10
Chronicity of Neglect 11
Race, Culture, and Community 11
Forms of Neglect 15
Physical Neglect 15
Emotional Neglect 17
Failure to Thrive (FTT) 18
Other Forms 21
Models of Child Maltreatment 23
Medical_Diagnostic 23
Sociological 24
Legal 24
Ecological 24
Developmental_Ecological 25
Parent-Focused Vs. Child-Focused 26
Assessment Tools 28
Limitations of the Research 31
Personalistic Causes: Gender 33
Personalistic Causes: Mental/Psychological 34
Personalistic Causes: Substance Abuse 37
Personalistic Causes: The Child 37
The Cycle Theory: Intergenerational Transmission
of Maltreatment 39
Economic Causes: Poverty 41
Environmental Causes: Multiple Pathways 45
Environmental Causes: Social Isolation 45
Environmental Factors: Culture 47
Effects of Neglect 49
Prevention and Treatment 53
Conclusion 61
Appendix A: Child Neglect Index 67
Appendix B: List of Acronyms 71
Bibliography 73
Introduction
In October 1998, the FVPU invited Canadian experts in child abuse issues to meet to help Health Canada and its partners set future directions in preventing child abuse. One of the more pressing areas that the committee recommended be researched was the issue of "neglect." Dr. Paul Steinhauer, renowned for his work in this field, emphasized the importance of "neglect" especially in the early years of child development.
Infant's brains are immature at birth and do not reach full maturity until the age of two. This is an extremely sensitive and critical period in the child's development. During this period, certain areas of the brain show heightened sensitivity to stimulation. Severe or chronic neglect also does long term damage to brain development with the result being lifelong changes to the individual's ability to regulate thought, emotions, and behaviour.
In Canada, the definitions of abuse and neglect differ among jurisdictions. In undertaking the Canadian Incidence Study on Reported Child Abuse and Neglect, a multi-stage survey design was used because there is a broad array of agency information, variations in definitions of child abuse and neglect, and inconsistent file recording standards. This document builds on past work and complements current activities in the area of child neglect.
This report summarizes research definitions and child welfare models for the prevention and treatment of child neglect. The review was limited to relevant research published between 1993 and 1998. Issues related to child neglect reveal interesting insights into the current challenges facing child welfare practitioners and researchers in Canada.
The FVPU has prepared a companion document to this report, Child Neglect: Promising Approaches, which will be an overview of current child welfare legislation, prevention, intervention, and treatment programs.
To better understand how we arrived at the current situation, a short review of the past is illustrative. A Brief History of Child Maltreatment Research
Douglas Barnett et al. (1993) described the American experience:
In an effort to deter economic destitution, separation of poor children from their families was encouraged prior to the 20th century. In these cases, parents were thought to promote poverty and dependency in their children through their examples of "laziness." These ethics were slow to change. Not until the turn of the current century were distinctions made between neglectful parents and impoverished parents (p. 11).
In Canada,
[T]he category of neglect owes its existence to a set of class
relations that allowed middle class reformers in the earlier stages
of industrial capitalist development to apply legal sanctions to
particular parents who occupied marginal positions vis-à-vis
the larger economy. Reformers viewed these marginalized families,
which were often headed by women, as unproductive, and they saw
in them two potential threats, about which they were explicit. One
was fear of the "contamination" of their own children
via exposure to the children of these families in the school system.
The second was concern about the potential long-term expense to
society of citizens poorly fitted out for productive membership
in the labour force. Child welfare legislation provided the grounds
for intervention into and rearrangement of these families, while
simultaneously preserving the ideal of the private home and family
for those who conformed to their own beliefs and standards (Swift,
1995a, p. 85).
A social reform movement in the 1880s and 1890s addressed itself to the issue of improving living conditions for deprived children. One result of this movement was the establishment of the first Children's Aid Society in Canada, which was founded in Toronto in 1891.
A child who is found begging or receiving alms.
A child who is found wandering about without any home or proper guardianship.
A child who is found associating or dwelling with a thief, drunkard, or vagrant, and growing up without salutary parental control.
A child who is found in any house of ill-fame or the company of a reputed prostitute.
A child who is found destitute, being an orphan or having a surviving parent undergoing punishment for crime.
(Swift, 1995a, p. 41)
Swift pointed out that this early definition addresses evidence of poverty, the need to care for the child, and moral issues concerning the activities of caretakers and guardians. According to American researchers Rose and Meezan (1993), the moral aspect of child neglect has become less important over time (Swift, 1995a, p. 41).
Child neglect was the original concept behind Canadian child welfare legislation. Swift noted:
[I]t remained the primary organizing idea in child welfare work until the 1960s. With the publication of work by Kempe et al. (1962) on "the battered child syndrome," neglect began to assume a lower profile as the public and social workers responded to this far more dramatic idea of maltreatment. The identification of the "syndrome," according to Hutchinson (1990) accounted for quick passage of extra funding and mandatory reporting requirements in the United States, with Canadian jurisdictions following suit. This narrow definition of child maltreatment was soon broadened to include aspects of neglect, which then reappeared as subcategories of abuse.... The subsuming of neglect into abuse also reflects a repriorization of problems in the daily practice of child welfare work (Swift, 1995a, p. 43).
The "subsuming of neglect into abuse" is described by Rose and Meezan (1993):
All child maltreatment was considered as a single phenomenon until
1964, when Leontine Young published her landmark study of families
known to child welfare agencies. Young attempted to make clear distinctions
between abusive and
neglectful parents using three factors: interpersonal traits, the
intent of the parent to maltreat a child, and the effects of maltreatment.
Wolock and Horowitz coined the phrase "the neglect of neglect" in 1984 and discussed the inattention to child neglect by both professionals and the media. The reasons that have been cited for this inattention to child neglect follow:
1. Some believe that neglect
does not result in serious consequences.
2. Many may feel that it is inappropriate to judge parents involved in poverty-related neglect.
3. Many may be reluctant to become involved in child neglect because the problem seems insurmountable.
4. Some may find other forms of maltreatment more compelling.
5. Ambiguity and vagueness regarding what constitutes neglect cause confusion.
6. Child neglect provokes negative feelings.
(Dubowitz 1994) (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 109).
The neglect of neglect, while now a cliché, is still true.
[J]ournals covering child maltreatment include few articles on child neglect; in 1993, 6% of the papers in Child Abuse and Neglect were on neglect. Also, in 1993, fewer than 2% of federally funded research studies on child maltreatment focused on child neglect (Interagency Research Committee, 1994) (Dubowitz, 1994, p. 556).
In both Canada and the United States, child protection services have been struggling to cope with a dramatic increase in the demand for services, believed to be due largely to heightened public awareness of maltreatment issues resulting in more reporting of suspected cases of child maltreatment to authorities in the 1980s and 1990s. One way in which it appears that increased demand has been dealt with in the United States is a narrowing of the definition of child neglect. According to Dubowitz (1994, p. 556), it appears that child neglect is not a clinical priority and except for very severe or life-threatening cases, it is screened out at intake by overburdened child protection services.
However, the most recent national
incidence statistics in the United States
appear to contradict this view. The U.S.
Third National Incidence Study of Child
Abuse and Neglect (NIS-3) notes that the
1993 study found large increases in the
number of children suffering from
emotional neglect and physical neglect
compared with the 1986 study, NIS-2.
The estimated number of children
who suffered Harm Standard
emotional neglect in 1993 was
Even less clear are the trends in the Canadian statistics. "In most Canadian jurisdictions, official statistics on reported child neglect do not exist" (Trocmé, 1996, p. 152). The federal government, in concert with the provinces and territories, has begun to address this gap in knowledge by initiating the Canadian Incidence Study of Reported Child Abuse and Neglect. Until the study reports its findings, however, Canadian researchers rely on the limited statistics available in this country and theorize what trends might be evident here.
Recent research confirms that neglect cases remain the largest single category of cases processed in Canadian child welfare offices (Trocmé et al., 1994; Federal-Provincial Working Group, 1994) (Swift, 1995a, p. 67).
Statistics collected in Quebec in 1991 found the following proportions
among 12,256 retained cases: 77% were
neglect, 10% were physical abuse, and 13% were sexual abuse. The
United States has reported similar statistics, although with a higher
proportion of neglect cases among all cases of child maltreatment.
Among the cases of physical abuse and neglect cases reported to
the New York State Central Registry of Child Abuse in 1988, 93%
were for child neglect (Palacio-Quintin et al., 1993, p. 154).
In the United States, the definition of child neglect has become narrower since 1980, according to Giovannoni (1993). She noted that neglect currently comprises about 50% of child maltreatment reports in the United States, compared to about 80% of reports 15 years ago (p. 8). According to the 1988 U.S. National Incidence Study, almost 43% of identified neglect
Based on a 50 state survey of CPS [child protection services] agencies
in the US, an estimated 2.7 million children were reported as victims
of child abuse and neglect in 1990. Of those, approximately 45%
were reported for neglect, as compared to 25% for physical abuse,
16% for sexual abuse, 6% for emotional maltreatment (some of whom
experience
emotional neglect), and 8% for "other" forms of maltreatment
(NCCAN, 1992). Approximately 40% of reported neglect cases are substantiated
(Erickson & Egeland, 1996,
p. 8).
The wide variation in statistical trends and the facility with which agencies may broaden or narrow assessments of child neglect can be attributed to a host of definitional issues confronting child neglect research and intervention.
Definitions of child abuse and neglect are not static phenomena, nor do they reflect issues that will be resolved in the decades to come (D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 16).
Definitions influence the way the issue of child neglect is conceptualized for research, reporting, understanding the causes, and formulating intervention and prevention strategies. As with other forms of child maltreatment, child neglect research has many grey areas, characterized by a widespread lack of consensus. On the "front lines" of child welfare practice, workers probably do not have much time to indulge in scholarly debates about definition. By virtue of necessity, they frequently rely on personal discretion and professional judgement.
In fact, there appears to be some impatience with definitional matters:
After many years of professional involvement in working with emotionally abused and neglected children and their families the author became convinced that the term "maltreatment" is the most appropriate to describe all forms of child abuse and neglect.... [T]endencies among professionals to compartmentalize aspects of the problem and place them in watertight compartments create their own problems, as there are more similarities than differences in the various characteristics and manifestations of abuse (Iwaniec, 1995, p. 189).
In reality, however, human processes are complex and our understanding of the causes and effects of certain behaviours changes over time. The "neglect of neglect" is seen in part as a result of a lack of definitional clarity and encourages the tendency of researchers to confound child abuse and child neglect. Most researchers indicate that abuse and neglect are distinct forms of child maltreatment and that there is a growing consensus that each requires specific and different interventions to
Increasingly, researchers are trying to differentiate between abuse and neglect and to better define the terms used in the field of child welfare in order to further the understanding of child maltreatment in general. Definitional clarity is needed, especially since child welfare and child protection services involve practitioners and researchers from a variety of backgrounds, disciplines, and perspectives. Child welfare's definitional issues are operationa-lized on a daily basis by police, pediatricians, legislators, and child protection workers. In addition, the system has considerable power over the families involved with child protection services. Not everyone is comfortable with having children removed from their homes based on a child welfare worker's personal judgement about a parent's suitability or a subjective interpretation of "neglect."
To implement the public agenda of protecting children from harm,
definitional specificity has become increasingly necessary for making
systematic and relatively objective decisions about when intervention
into family life is warranted
(D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 8).
Some current definitions of child neglect follow:
A condition in which a caretaker responsible for the child, either deliberately or by extraordinary inattentiveness, permits the child to experience avoidable present suffering and/or fails to provide one or more of the ingredients generally deemed essential for developing a person's physical, intellectual, and emotional capacities (Gaudin, 1993a, pp. 3_4).
Child neglect is the term used most often to encompass parents' or caretakers' failure to provide basic physical health care, supervision, nutrition, personal hygiene, emotional nurturing, education, or safe housing. It also includes child abandonment or expulsion, and custody-related forms of inattention to the child's needs (Gaudin, 1993b, p. 67).
There are parents who continually fail to provide for their children's needs, and usually in many ways. These failures eventually affect the child's health and/or development adversely. Characteristically, these parents do not feel guilt over their omissions, and often simply fail to recognize the harmful consequences of the chronic neglected state of their children (Hall et al., 1982, p. 6), (Swift, 1995a, pp. 70_71).
Generally, child neglect means the failure of a parent or a caretaker responsible for the child's care to provide minimally adequate food, clothing, shelter, supervision, and/or medical care for the child. Defining "minimally adequate" levels of care, and reaching consensus on these definitions, however, are not easy processes (Gaudin, 1993a, p. 1).
Zuravin (cited in Nelson, Saunders, and Landsman, 1990) after analyzing definitional issues of neglect, concluded that most definitions agree it is an act of omission "judged by a mixture of community values and professional expertise to be inappropriate and damaging" and as "failure to perform parental duties related to supervision and physical needs of the child" (Downs et al., 1996, p. 182).
Giovannoni (1993) noted that in practical social policy terms, child neglect is "behaviour by parents or responsible caretakers that warrants
intrusion into the family's
privacy and autonomy, and
the expenditure of social
resources to remedy the
behaviour."
(p. 8)
Numerous definitions of child neglect have been proposed by researchers and practitioners. The definitional debates spring from the lack of consensus on answers to the following questions:
What are the indispensable, minimally adequate types of care that children require?
What actions or failures to act on the part of the parents or other caretaker constitute neglectful behaviour?
Must the parents' or caretaker's action or inaction be intentional, willful or not?
What are the effects of the actions or inactions on the
child's health, safety, and
development?
Is the family's situation a result of poverty, or a result of parental neglect?
(Gaudin, 1993a, p. 3)
The last question reflects one of the major controversies surrounding current conceptualizations. As articulated by Giovannoni in 1982, "Is `it' poverty or is `it' psychopathology?" (Swift, 1995, p. 88)
There is undisputed evidence that the incidence of neglect is more prevalent in areas of extreme poverty. Some researchers note that not all children living in such conditions are neglected, however, and they conclude that poverty may contribute to neglect, but it does not define it.
Pelton (1997), on the other hand, pointed out "that child neglect usually has multiple causes and, like accidental injury, is strongly related to low socio-economic status. Most injuries to children in child protection cases are not intentional. The emphasis on parental responsibility and on child protection laws, policies, and practices has contributed to excessive placement of children in foster care and to insufficient emphasis on directing resources toward remedying dangerous conditions and poverty associated with unintentional injuries and severe harm" (p. 7).
Severity of Consequences
Legal advocates insist on clear evidence of serious harm before court intervention. Research indicates that when child welfare workers are determining whether abuse or neglect exists in a particular situation, the actual focus is on parental omissions in care that are likely to increase the risk of harm to the child.
In a study published in 1979, Giovannoni and Becerra examined the views of both professionals and lay people in response to vignettes of parental care. They found that the main criterion used in defining whether abuse or neglect existed was the seriousness of impact upon the child, and that this criterion was fairly consistent across groups. Trocmé and Tam's research (1994, p. 16) supported this view, showing increased likelihood that a case will be substantiated if "the presence of any form of harm or risk of harm" to a child is shown (Swift, 1995a, p. 70).
A given behaviour can be interpreted as neglectful or not depending on the severity of the consequences to the child, the duration and frequency of neglect, as well as the cultural context in which the behaviour occurs (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 110).
Crouch and Milner (1993) argued that severity is an important but overlooked variable. Severity is typically assessed according to the magnitude of outcomes to children or the degree of demonstrable harm.
Some negative outcomes are difficult to measure, such as emotional consequences. Some consequences are neither immediate nor short term.
Thus, in 1988 the Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS) added
"endangered" as a category for children who demonstrated
no present evidence of injury but for whom future risk of injury is
a reasonable risk. One difficulty in considering potential harm is
predicting the likelihood that harm will actually occur and whether
that potential harm is significant (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 110).
Chronicity of Neglect
Research indicates that frequent and repeated deficits in child care are more likely to be considered neglectful. Dubowitz, DHHS, and Zuravin have argued that frequency and chronicity should be evaluated in the context of the severity of harm involved in a particular act, as a single omission can have serious consequences and "an omission in care that harms or endangers a child constitutes neglect, whether it occurs once or a hundred times" (Dubowitz, Black, Starr, et al., 1993) (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 111).
Chronically neglectful families are typically multi-problem families with pervasive deficits in knowledge, skills and tangible resources, whereas nonchronically neglectful families have experienced recent life crises that have overwhelmed normally sufficient coping strategies (Gaudin, 1993b, p. 68).
Dubowitz et al. (1993) noted that although estimates of severity are typically based on the degree of harm involved, this is not always immediately apparent and/or easy to assess. Potential harm is more controversial than actual harm and professionals have been reluctant to rate a situation as maltreatment unless actual harm was evident (Gelles, 1982) (Dubowitz et al., 1993, p. 17).
According to Dubowitz et al. (1993), research has demonstrated long-term psychological harm resulting from neglect and, given that human nature and life inevitably involve some degree of risk taking, it makes little sense to see every instance of potential harm as neglect. Helping families minimize risks is important and potential harm should be included in a definition of neglect (p. 17). Race,
Culture, and Community
Zuravin et al. (1996) have noted that most current definitions of child neglect contain an element (recognized or not) of cultural values. As newer conceptual models tend to acknowledge the existence of different cultural and social values in both Canada and the United States, researchers have tried to identify and classify some of these differences. It should be noted that researchers have stressed the methodological limits and potential biases in this early research and have cautioned readers about drawing sweeping conclusions from the following, somewhat limited, results.
In an effort to determine how cultural and community values might vary, Polansky compared perceptions of
Rose and Meezan (1996) explored the perceptions of the seriousness of specific components of neglect held by mothers from three cultural groups (Caucasian, African- American, Latino) with public child welfare workers in Chicago. Their findings suggest "that members of minority groups perceive some types of child neglect as more serious than child welfare workers and workers of all types see neglect as less serious than the mothers" (p. 140). This led Rose and Meezan (1996) to make the following conclusion:
The findings of the study seem to suggest that the practice of hiring investigators who lack social work backgrounds in the protective service system should be reviewed.... They view child neglect incidents as significantly more serious than do service workers, and seem to operate with a broader definition of child neglect than their service worker counterparts. Their behaviour may thus be contributing to the current overload of the child protection services system (p. 157).
Knudsen pointed to the significance of community perceptions of child neglect since it "is primarily nonprofessionals who identify, and thereby define, what events constitute child maltreatment." In addition, Knudsen and others noted that approximately one third to one half of all reports to child protection services by lay people are considered founded reports of maltreatment (Barnett et al., 1993, p. 25).
Dubowitz et al. (1998) compared views on child neglect among African-American and White community members of middle and low socio-economic status and with child maltreatment professionals' views. There were small but significant differences: both middle-class community groups expressed greater concern for psychological care than the lower-class African-American group. Both groups of African-Americans were more concerned than Whites about physical care.
Overall, there is considerable agreement among the community
Discussion of race and culture in research conducted in other countries may be of limited application to the Canadian situation. However, child welfare in Canada has been marked by unique cultural biases. As Swift (1995a) has noted, child welfare is
...a system well suited to keeping order, the order required for existing power and economic relations to be maintained, and child neglect is a concept well suited to justifying processes through which order is maintained. Child welfare is not, however, a system well suited to meeting the needs of Aboriginal people; surely several decades of destructive outcomes resulting from our efforts provide sufficient evidence of this. Nor is neglect a category that actually serves Aboriginal people or saves Aboriginal children, although it may appear this way in individual cases. Child welfare work with Aboriginal people, in fact, illustrates very well the way Althusser's concepts of repressive and ideological apparatuses operate in concert to produce desired effects, purposes captured in the phrase "teaching Mom a lesson." Bureaucratic processes work simultaneously to produce the appearance of equal treatment and "business as usual" for workers in the system. Workers apply the schema of neglect in more or less the same way to specific families and, most of the time, are unable to see beyond their own fragmented work processes to observe the part this classification process plays in the subjugation and racialization of the whole group (pp. 147_148).
Different researchers have proposed a wide range of different subtypes of child neglect. Rose and Meezan, and Swift have noted that until recently, physical neglect predominated as the first concern. Swift (1995a) commented:
This concern is mirrored in practice; as we will see, it is usually the traditional physical signs of neglect that predominate in both case records and in workers' talk about neglect (p. 72).
In the United States, state legislation "solidified the idea that the lack of adequate food, clothing, shelter, medical care and supervision, or abandonment were the cornerstones of a definition of neglect." Specific references to emotional well-being were not included in the legislative definitions of neglect until the early 1970s (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 281). This appears to be similar to the Canadian experience.
The following are some examples of the different categories of child neglect that have been used by researchers in the past 20 years:
Becker et al. (1995) noted that Giovannoni and Becerra divided
neglectful behaviours into four categories:
educational neglect
abandonment
failure to provide
fostering delinquency
(p. 29)
Becker et al. (1995) further noted
K. Miller's four classifications of neglect:
educational neglect
medical neglect
intentional drugging
abandonment
(p. 29)
These subcategories of neglect were suggested by Falconer and Swift:
physical
medical
education
supervision and guidance
abandonment
(Swift, 1995a, p. 72)
Zuravin and Taylor later proposed eight categories of neglect:
lack of physical health care
lack of medical health care
child abandonment
shelter hazards
lack of household sanitation
lack of hygiene
lack of nutrition
(Crouch & Milner, 1993, p. 50)
Daro listed the following as typologies of neglect:
physical neglect
deprivation of necessities
educational neglect/deprivation
medical care neglect
intentional drugging
abandonment/lack of supervision
failure to provide
fostering delinquency (Becker et al., 1995, p. 29)
Hegar and Yungman proposed three main categories of neglect:
Physical deprivation of basics, such as clothing, shelter, hygiene
Developmentaldeprivation of experiences necessary for growth and development, including supervision, education, medical and mental health care
Emotionalincludes
a) General emotional neglect: parental incapacity to recognize the child's need for attention, security, self-esteem, and to recognize emotional needs
b) Non-organic FTT (failure to thrive)
(Palacio-Quintin & Éthier, 1993, pp. 155_156)
According to O. Barnett et al., 1997, at least 11 subtypes of neglect have been "consistently described":
health care neglect
personal hygiene neglect
nutritional neglect
neglect of household safety
neglect of household sanitation
inadequate shelter
abandonment
supervisory neglect
educational neglect
emotional neglect
fostering delinquency (p. 112)
The current definition of child neglect used by the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services when conducting national incidence studies of child maltreatment is probably the most well-known categorization in North America. Gaudin (1993) reported that the subcategories of neglect, according to the second National Incidence and Prevalence of Child Abuse and Neglect Study, are
Physical neglect:
refusal of health care
delay in health care
abandonment
other custody issues
other physical neglect
Supervision:
inadequate supervision
Emotional neglect:
inadequate nurturance/affection
chronic/extreme abuse or domestic violence
permitted drug/alcohol abuse
permitted other maladaptive behaviour
refusal of psychological care
delay in psychological care
other emotional neglect
Educational neglect:
permitted chronic truancy
failure to enrol or other truancy
inattention to special education need
It is notable that "excluded from these definitions are cases where the parent was financially unable to provide reasonable safe, hygienic living conditions" (Gaudin, 1993a, p. 6).
According to the 1988 U.S. National Incidence Study (NIS-2), almost 43% of identified neglect was physical neglect, 36.6% was inadequate supervision, and 20.8% was failure or delay in providing health care (Gaudin, 1993a, p. 6).
Emotional Neglect
Despite the apparently few substantiated reports of emotional neglect in the United States, Swift noted that "emotional issues have begun to take the stage in recent years" (1995a, p. 72). The greatest disagreement exists over emotional neglect.
Although most experts agree on broad conceptual parameters of emotional
neglect that include failure to provide support, security, and encouragement,
they disagree on the operationa-lization of such behaviours
(O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 112).
Emotional neglect is specifically mentioned in child protection legislation in some North American jurisdictions. According to Rose and Meezan (1993), its inclusion was based on the assertion that a child's early emotional care affected later behaviour and psychological adjustment (p. 283).
Garbarino, Guttman and Seeley used multi-dimensional scaling with a combination of categories of psychological abuse and neglect to identify and empirically define five distinct subtypes of psychological maltreatment. The five subtypes are spurning, terrorizing, isolating, exploiting/corrupting, and denying emotional responsiveness. However, this definition has not yet been universally accepted in the field of maltreatment (Becker et al., 1995, p. 28).
...is an extension or replacement of moral neglect, according to
Gordon, but a more "psycholo-gical and scientific" category
than moral neglect. The term "emotional neglect" has been
in use by social workers since the 1950s, was codified by 1960,
but has eluded the specificity required for wide legal usage (Gordon,
1988, p. 162). Polansky et al. brought emotional neglect to the
forefront as a child welfare issue in the Childhood Level of Living
(CLL) Scale [a tool used to assess the existence of child neglect];
36 per cent of the items in this measure concern emotional or cognitive
care of children (Gordon, 1988, p. 163) (Swift, 1995a, p. 72).
The lack of definitional clarity and overlap that characterizes neglect in general is amplified within discussions of emotional neglect in particular (or "psychologically unavailable parenting" as Erickson and Egeland [1996] refer to emotional neglect). If Reid et al. (1994) are right in saying that "[i]n effect, neglect is a residual category composed of all instances of child maltreatment other than those explicitly defined as sexual, physical and emotional abuse" (p. 12), then perhaps the reason why so many seemingly disparate typologies get lumped under the heading of "emotional neglect" is that it is the residual form within a residual category. For example, O. Barnett et al. (1997, p. 112) noted considerable overlap between definitions of emotional neglect and psychological maltreatment.
One attempt to define emotional neglect refers to it as
...the passive ignoring of a child's emotional needs; to lack of attention and of stimulation; and to parental unavailability to care, to supervise, to guide, to teach, and to protect.... Emotional neglect more often than not originates from parental unawareness and ignorance, depressive moods, chaotic life-styles, poverty, lack of support, and lack of appropriate child-rearing models (often based on parental childhood experiences), unwittingly impairing child-development and well-being (Iwaniec, 1995, p. 5).
Failure to Thrive (FTT)
Many terms used in discussions of child maltreatment are either poorly defined or very fluid. An illustration of the kind of confusion that can result is reflected in an examination of the research literature on "failure to thrive" (FTT).
FTT syndrome has been defined as delayed physical growth due to
inadequate emotional care; however, this is difficult to prove empirically
and the definition is not universally accepted (Rose & Meezan,
1993,
pp. 282_283).
The first cases of the "syndrome" were diagnosed in the 1940s among institutionalized infants. It was seen to be the result of psychosocial deprivation of early childhood development in institutions. Researchers surmised that infants living in unstimulating homes must be subject to equivalent disadvantage.
Their task was then to demonstrate that the primary caretaker of
the child with NOFT [Non-organic Failure to Thrive] (invariably
the mother) was not capable of providing an adequate parenting experience
by reason of psychiatric disorder, poverty, or marital and family
discord (Puckering et al., 1995,
p. 574).
FTT appears to be the result of insufficient attachment between the mother and the baby. Explanations for the lack of bonding include early deprivation of the mother during her own childhood, difficulties during the pregnancy or childbirth resulting in prematurity or congenital defects, acute illness of mother or baby, and stressful current life events (Mayhall & Norgard, 1983) (Downs et al., 1996, p. 189).
According to DiLeonardi (1993):
[T]he traditional definition of "failure-to-thrive" has changed dramatically over the years. In the 1970s, Barbero et al. (1975) described failure-to-thrive as being diagnosable by a series of symptoms including weight below the third centile for age with subsequent weight gain in the presence of appropriate nurturing; no evidence of systemic disease or abnormality; developmental retardation with subsequent acceleration when the environment is changed; clinical signs of maternal deprivation; and presence of significant environmental psychosocial disruption. It was regarded as a severe and life-threatening form of neglect in many jurisdictions. Currently, failure-to-thrive is diagnosed as weight below the fifth centile with no organic reason and is not regarded as child neglect (Dubowitz, Black, Starr Jr. and Zuravin, 1993). There are no developmental markers. This change in definition has increased the number of failure-to-thrive cases geometrically, since by this
Hanson (1993) hypothesized that Non-organic Failure to Thrive (NFTT), which has no biological origin,
...may be caused by parental inexperience in not knowing how to feed properly or how much babies eat, or by diluting the formula for lack of money. NFTT can also be a more deeply rooted problem. Infants quickly sense the feelings and attitudes of caregivers. If the parent feels ambivalence or hostility toward the infant, lacks attachment, or sees the child as too demanding, the infant may react negatively.... Maternal deprivation has largely been held accountable for the NFTT syndrome. Family systems proponents suggest this condition is symptomatic of total family maladaptation, of family disengagement characterized by distancing and lack of communication within the total family unit (p. 105).
Others have reached different conclusions:
However, with the increasing use of well controlled studies, and the objective measurement of family relationships using instruments of known psychometric properties, it became clear that within the syndrome of NOFT [Non-organic Failure to Thrive] not all families are alike, and parents do not consistently fit into these limited categories (e.g., Drotar, 1991) (Puckering et al., 1995, p. 573).
FTT cases are believed to result from "psychosocial diseases" such as physical neglect and psychological maltreatment. "Although most experts agree that non-organic FTT results from psychosocial difficulties that reduce caloric intake, the nature of the psychosocial difficulties has been the subject of considerable debate." Some see it as a medical condition due to physical child neglect (inadequate food and nutrition). Others focus on psychological aspects of FTT, such as isolation and lack of stimulation and view FTT as a psychological condition. Studies evaluating the differences between non-organic FTT infants and normally developing infants, for example, have found that the interactions between FTT children and their mothers are characterized by deficits in attachment, sensitivity toward the child, and degree of comfort between mother and child (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 118).
Controversies surrounding non-organic failure-to-thrive children continue unabated. It is arguable that the assumption
that FTT is the result only of neglect is unsupported: the condition
may involve negative parental feelings, which are demonstrated by
difficult parent-child interaction, and indeed there may be many other
factors that need to be taken into account including unwanted pregnancy,
insecure attachment, problematic temperamental attribute of the child,
distorted parental perceptions and attitudes, and insufficient support
systems for parents in need (Iwaniec, 1995,
p. 189).
Other Forms
Concern with inadequate medical care has evolved from an emphasis on not providing care to actively refusing care, most often based on religious conviction (Rose & Meezan, 1993).
Withholding medically indicated treatment from newborn babies with serious birth defects is another "new" and somewhat controversial category of neglect, as is prenatal exposure to drugs/alcohol (Gaudin, 1993a, p. 8).
Neglect of the unborn child, or "prenatal neglect," is another recently identified and controversial form of neglect. It is generally considered to include actions that occur during the prenatal period that can potentially harm the unborn child. In current practice, the focus is on women who abuse drugs or alcohol during pregnancy. In 1993, the U.S. National Committee to Prevent Child Abuse found 6,922 infants were reported for prenatal drug exposure in 1993; 7,469 in 1994 (Wiese and Daro, 1995) (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 113).
There is limited research evidence linking prenatal drug exposure and negative developmental outcome. Nonetheless, in 1994, 27 states in the US required the reporting of drug-exposed babies and according to O. Barnett et al. (1997), it appears more legal responses are emerging. There is disagreement over the rights of the unborn versus the rights of the mother, and punitive responses to substance-abusing pregnant women have been questioned on practical, constitutional, therapeutic, and empirical grounds.
Although the problem of prenatal neglect continues to be the focus of much theoretical discussion and empirical research, solutions will likely remain elusive for some time (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 113).
Research also has clearly revealed that each perspective, by itself, is insufficient for accounting for the causes and effects of child maltreatment. Rather, an integration of approaches seems most warranted. Consequently, these different viewpoints should not be thought of as mutually exclusive nor should they be inappropriately or artificially pitted against one another (D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 21).
Some of the main conceptual models that are current in child neglect research today are outlined below.
Medical_Diagnostic
The medical diagnostic approach to child maltreatment is the oldest model outlined here. This model has a narrow focus and is limited primarily to the most severe instances of child maltreatment, such as those that can be documented by x-rays of children with multiple fractures (D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 18). This approach has resulted in formulations that emphasize treating a disorder afflicting the parent (e.g., the battered child syndrome). Psychiatric or psychological conceptualizations that focus on perpetrator characteristics are a feature of the research.
[C]urrent knowledge about neglect has been limited almost entirely to questions about the supposed causes of neglect and effectiveness of various treatment models. It is, in other words, a highly individualistic approach, based on a view of neglect as a disease entity in need of a cure. This approach assumes the continuing need for the current array of social services and programs. That these programs have not reduced the scope of the problem
over the past hundred years (Nagi, 1977; Rose & Meezan, 1993) is not addressed (Swift, 1995a, p. 11).
Sociological
Sociological models of child maltreatment (e.g., Gelles, Giovannoni
and Becerra) define child maltreatment in terms of a social judgement
of parental acts that are deemed inappropriate by cultural standards
and practices. Sociological models focus on contextual conditions,
such as poverty, that give rise to mal-
treatment.
In accord with the sociological perspective, we propose that the emphasis in defining child maltreatment should be on the parental acts that are viewed to be unacceptable or "improper" by society, because the majority of the population believe that they place children at risk for physical and emotional harm. We emphasize parental actions over other variables to define child maltreatment for a number of reasons (D. Barnett et al., 1993, pp. 22_23).
Legal
Legal models of child maltreatment (e.g., Wald) establish clear guidelines regarding parental actions justifying court action. These models aim to provide national standards for judicial decisions about maltreatment.
Ecological
Ecological models (e.g., Bronfenbrenner, Belsky, Garbarino, Starr, Zuravin) place equal emphasis on environmental and familial contributions to maltreatment. These models maintain that society as a whole shares responsibility for child protection (D. Barnett et al., 1993, pp. 18_19). Family behaviour is viewed within the larger social context in which it is embedded.
Current theory emphasizes the ecology of childhood, with multiple and interacting factors contributing to the occurrence of child abuse and neglect. In addition to the individual and interpersonal factors, interactions between parents and children are influenced by community and society factors, such as the availability of child care and poverty (Dubowitz et al., 1993, p. 10).
Dubowitz et al. (1993) addressed three issues of ecological context: parental understanding of the needs of children, the relevance of cultural or religious beliefs, and the role of poverty. These researchers noted that parents sometimes need information from professionals to understand children's needs and that society shares an obligation for children's care. Some circumstances (e.g., sending children to school) are parents' responsibility, whereas other situations require professionals to inform parents (e.g., lead poisoning).
However, if reasonable efforts have been made to inform parents about
their children's needs and parents are not responsive, they are not
fulfilling their responsibility and they are contributing to their
child's neglect (p. 19).
Developmental_Ecological
Belsky (1993), who has written an excellent review of the scientific research into the causes of maltreatment, used a "developmental_ecological" analysis, which underscores the dyadic nature of problematic parenting.
If, as is now widely acknowledged, maltreatment is a transactional by-product of processes taking place between parent and child in a family and community context, than studies...that examine "main effects" of child characteristics are more likely to underestimate the interactive role that factors like prematurity and handicap play (p. 419).
Belsky noted that the current research points to a model in which child maltreatment is the result of a transactional process involving the characteristics of children and parents, embedded in multiple contexts. Cicchetti and Toth (1995) concurred:
We believe that a developmental framework holds considerable promise
for helping to unravel
not only the sequelae of maltreatment, but also the processes that
underlie these maladaptive outcomes. Therefore, in accord with a
developmental psychopathology perspective, we approach our review
of the consequences of maltreatment within an organizational framework
that necessitates an examination of adaptation on stage-salient
issues
(p. 546).
In its 1989 publication, Research on Children and Adolescents
with Mental, Behavioral, and Development Disorders, the U.S.
Institute of Medicine noted that a developmental approach should
take into account "the emerging behavioural repertoire, cognitive
and language functions, social and emotional processes, and changes
occurring in anatomical structures and physiological processes of
the brain" throughout the life course (Cicchetti & Toth,
1995,
p. 542).
A developmental psychopathology approach to defining maltreatment is also proposed by D. Barnett et al. (1993).
Consummate to understanding the multifaceted nature of the causes and consequence of maltreatment is the recognition of the developmental aspects of child abuse and neglect. Each of the components involved in child maltreatment, the environment, the parent, and the child, are transacting over time....
As well as by Cicchetti and Toth (1995):
Finally, in a recent developmental_ecological analysis of the etiology of child abuse and neglect, Belsky (1993) examines a variety of contexts of maltreatment, including the contributions of parent and child characteristics and pro-cesses, parenting and parent child interactions, and community, cultural, and evolutionary contexts of maltreatment. After a careful and thorough analysis of the research literature, Belsky (1993) concludes that child abuse and neglect are multiply determined by factors that are operating at various levels of the ecology. As such, it is impossible to unearth a single pathway to maltreatment or to consistently identify a factor or factors that, when present, culminate in an act of maltreatment. Rather, Belsky's review confirms that maltreatment occurs when stressors exceed supports and when risks are greater than protective factors (cf. Belsky, 1980; Cicchetti and Lynch, 1993; Cicchetti and Rizley, 1981). According to Belsky, the fact that research reviews consistently fail to identify etiological correlates of child maltreatment confirms the belief that etiological factors result in maltreatment only in combination with other contributory agents. Therefore, analyses that focus on "main effects" by comparing maltreated and comparison groups on a single variable may fail to reveal group differences because of an inability to elucidate the interplay among factors. These points are important to keep in mind when evaluating research on the sequelae of child maltreatment (pp. 545_546).
Parent-Focused Vs. Child-Focused
Another conceptual approach for current research models divides definitions of child neglect (and more generally, child maltreatment) into two camps: "parent-focused" and "child-focused."
A widely prevalent framework in the child welfare field focuses on omissions in caregiver behaviour regarding children. Others are more concerned with basic needs of children not being met, whatever the reason (Dubowitz, 1994, p. 558).
Historically, the issue of neglect has been approached from the perspective of the parents' behaviour toward the child. As a result, there is relatively little research about the effects of neglect on children and few treatment interventions that focus on providing treatment services to the
The newer, child-focused approach is concerned with the effects of neglectful behaviour on the child.
This perspective arose in response to the potential abuses inherent in the emphasis on parental inaction: an overreliance on worker discretion and judgement as to the meaning and consequences of parental behaviour, placement of children in outside homes when substantial risk of harm was not clearly established, and cultural and class bias in the assessment of risk (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 284).
Goldstein, Freud and Solnit defined neglect from the point of view of harm to the child and placed their emphasis primarily on the child's psychological well-being. They defined neglect as "a lack of emotional attachment of the child to the parent based on the parent's failure to provide the attention a child needs to feel cared for, nourished, comforted, loved, and stimulated" (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 284).
Among those who have taken the parent-focused approach include Kadushin, who
...has argued that we must rely on parental behaviour as an indicator
of neglect because the effect of neglect may not always be immediately
visible....
Authors who favour this [broad] perspective have argued that judges
and caseworkers need discretion in determining neglect and that
more specific definitions remove such discretion. It has also been
argued that more specific definitions could not be sensitive to
community standards (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 283).
The parent-focused approach, it has been argued, allows workers to make use of protective services before evidence of harm has been demonstrated; therefore, a broader and more inclusive definition of neglect is desirable (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 287).
Dubowitz et al. (1993), on the other hand, stated that the focus of concern in defining neglect must be on children and their unmet needs because needs vary according to age and developmental level.
Neglect occurs when basic needs of children are not met, regardless of cause. Basic needs include adequate shelter, food, health care, clothing, education, protection, and nurturance (p. 10).
An evaluation of contributory factors is important for planning appropriate interventions, according to Dubowitz et al. (1993):
Situations where the effect on children is unclear should not be considered neglect; research
Narrow definitions, such as those used in legal and child protection systems, restrict our ability to fully understand neglect, according to Dubowitz et al., although they are relatively easy to operationalize and implement. Parental responsibility is implicit in narrow definitions, they say. Broad definitions may appear to absolve parents of all responsibility and are often vague and difficult to implement, but "despite the immense challenges associated with a broad conceptual definition of neglect, we suggest...that from a child's perspective this is a more meaningful and useful view" (Dubowitz et al., 1993, p. 10).
Assessment Tools
As previously mentioned, researchers have developed various measures to assist in assessing the existence of child neglect. Perhaps the best known of these is the Childhood Level of Living (CLL) Scale, which grew out of Polansky et al.'s 1967 research of neglectful mothers in rural Appalachia and was later applied to a study of low-income families in Philadelphia. The CLL Scale was designed to examine specific elements of care along a continuum from poor to excellent, in the arenas of physical, emotional, and cognitive care. This measure, which remains probably the most comprehensive available, allows for deficits in one area to be compensated for by strengths in another and for patterns of good as well as poor care to be identified (Swift, 1995a, p. 69).
Geared predominantly toward maternal carein a majority of households only the female parent was available for studythe CLL presents nine descriptive categories, five of which assess physical care and four the emotional, cognitive, or psychological factors. Under physical care, consideration is given to such facts as meal planning, medical care, safety issues, leaving the child alone, house or shelter adequacy and safety, appropriateness of sleeping and living conditions, and cleanliness. The psychological assessment considers the type of stimulation the child is given, the parents' emotional availability to the child, quality of discipline, the mother's concern for the child, and her own stability (Hally, Polansky, and Polansky, 1980) (Hanson, 1993, p. 96).
The CLL is lengthy and detailed (it includes 99 items). Some authors question the relevance of some of it and the cultural relevance for minorities.
Trocmé has developed the Ontario Child Neglect Index (CNI). The CNI was designed to be short, easy to administer and "accurately reflect child welfare practice within the framework of Ontario's child welfare laws" (Trocmé, 1996, p. 145).
The Child Neglect Index is designed to provide child welfare practitioners and researchers with a validated and easy-to-use instrument that specifies type and severity of neglect. Field testing shows that the validity and reliability of this one-page index compare favourably with the Child Well-Being Scales. Although the Child Neglect Index was designed within the context of Ontario's child welfare laws, the instrument and the structured expert-based method used to develop it can be easily applied to other jurisdictions (Trocmé, 1996, p. 145).
The CNI reflects Ontario's 1984 Child and Family Services Act, which defines neglect in terms of the different forms of physical or emotional harm that affect neglected children. This focus on evidence of harm or risk of harm marks an important conceptual shift in practice in Ontario, according to Trocmé. The shift is not as inclusive as the positions of Wald or Dubowitz who argued that neglect occurs when basic needs are not met, regardless of the cause. Instead, the legislation restricts the definition of neglect to situations in which harm to the child is associated with parental failure to "care or provide for" a child and excludes neglect attributed to other caregivers, such as school personnel or society at large as in cases of child poverty (Trocmé, 1996, p. 146).
Although in principle legislation should guide intervention, in practice this is not always the case. Given that only 10% of child protection cases are brought to court (Trocmé et al., 1994) practitioners exercise considerable discretion in deciding which situations should be targeted for service. The clinical factors considered by workers in making intervention decisions in child neglect cases may be quite different for cases that do not proceed to court. Workers involved in the field test raised this issue; they felt that the CNI was less sensitive to mild forms of child neglect. Although they recognized that the CNI reflected the legislation, they also believed that the legislation sets limits that fail to consider some families that require preventive
A copy of the Ontario CNI is included in Appendix A of this report.
Other assessment tools are used and adapted for research and practice. Another tool of note is the Strange Situation Procedure, which is used for assessing the quality of parent_infant attachment.
This assessment procedure, used most often when the child is 1 or 2 years of age, has been demonstrated to be a valid and reliable measure of the infant's adaptation within the context of the infant_caregiver relationship and it is predictive of the child's subsequent behaviour in a variety of situations (Erickson & Egeland, 1996, pp. 10_11).
family revenue below poverty line
mother sole financial provider
mother's first pregnancy occurs before the age of 21
four or more children in family
This study reported that 100% of negligent families and 84% of abusive families lived below the poverty line (Palacio-Quintin & Éthier, 1993, p. 156).
What determines whether maltreatment will take place is the balance of stressors and supports. When parents' stressors are stronger than the mitigating factors, maltreatment occurs. Current research reflects this understanding and etiological studies seek to identify "contributing rather than determining agents" (Belsky, 1993, p. 418).
Belsky noted that
...there is no shortage of causal agents that are invoked to explain the occurrence of physical child abuse and neglect. Some of the factors are historical (e.g., socie-tal attitudes toward family privacy) and some are contemporaneous (e.g., poverty); some are cultural (e.g., tolerance of violence) and some are situational (e.g., crying episode); and some are attributes of parents (e.g., hostile personality) and some of children (e.g., difficult temperament) (Belsky, 1993, p. 413).
Limitations of the Research
Etiological studies of child maltreatment have been criticized for a number of shortcomings in their scientific approach. Biased sampling, small samples, poorly matched control groups, and vagueness about severity and chronicity characterize many etiological studies of child maltreatment, according to Belsky (1993).
For the most part, studies of the etiology of child maltreatment lack precise a priori predictions
Belsky was careful to note that researchers are well aware of the
methodological difficulties but they are not easy to address and,
indeed, many research problems are
"fundamental to the study of child maltreatment."
Because there is no single cause of the physical abuse and neglect of children, and because these forms of maltreatment arise as a result of a transactional process involving characteristics of parents, children, and the multiple contexts in which they are embedded, the search for "main effects" invariably yields sporadic findings.... As Bronfenbrenner (1979) so astutely noted, in the ecology of human developmentand thus in the etiology of child maltreatment"the principal main effects are likely to be interactions" (p. 38) (Belsky, 1993, p. 414).
Contributing to the difficulties associated with studying child
abuse and neglect is the fact that researchers tend to base their
studies on families that have come to the attention of child protection
agencies and that have been labelled by the agencies as neglectful
or abusive. It is hard to say how reliable these labels are in
practice; many times, definitions are applied "after much negotiation
and consultation with the family, judicial authorities, and others"
(Belsky, 1993, p. 413). This reliance on "labels" of child
protection services is noteworthy:
Across the various subtypes of maltreatment, [Knudsen's] most consistent
finding was the absence of clear criteria for defining maltreatment
and systematizing the investigation and substantiation process.
Consequently, cases frequently were decided upon in an idio-
syncratic fashion guided primarily by the beliefs and practices
of the individual case workers (D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 26).
Nonetheless, research is conducted and reviewers point to improvements over time in how the work is conducted and reported. To present the research information on the causes of child neglect, we will borrow a framework from Polansky, who suggested that all causes of maltreatment can be grouped within three theories:
Personalisticattributing poor child care to individual differences among parental personalities, particularly their character structures.
Economicemphasizing the role of material deprivation and poverty.
Most research to date on the causes of child neglect focuses on
the personality characteristics of neglecting mothers. The reason
for the focus on mothers, as opposed to mothers and fathers, is
a subject of debate. Belsky's 1993 review noted that "virtually
all the research evidence examined here deals with mistreatment
of children by mothers. This should not imply anything other than
that mothers more often care for children than do fathers and thus
have been subject to more intensive investigations" (Belsky,
1993,
p. 414).
Swift, however, has a different
interpretation:
The discourse of neglect has long since established mothers as the "crucial variable" in neglect (Polansky et al., 1972), and this belief is echoed explicitly or implicitly by almost everyone writing about child neglect. The study of child neglect is in effect the study of mothers who "fail" (Swift, 1995a, p. 101).
Indeed, our society assigns prime responsibility for rearing children
to women and the research reflects that.
However, Swift (1995a) noted that the research obscures the fact
that it is analyzing mothers and mothering because the literature
frequently uses the terms "parents" and "parenting."
In fact, mother as the "crucial variable" is the main theme in most writing about neglect. Role rejection (Kadushin, 1967), lack of nurturing knowledge (Jones and McNeely, 1980), immaturity of the mother (Young, 1964; Katz, 1971), and the poor nurturing of the mother herself (Hall et al., 1982) all appear as variations on this theme. While many researchers are concerned with establishing the main causal variables of neglect, they also contribute to the definition of the problem by framing it in personal and intrafamilial terms (Swift, 1995a, p. 89).
As Swift went on to note, the "failure to provide care and in fact the complete abandonment of children by their fathers generally produces no comment at all" in case workers' files.
In cases of neglect, fathers are usually not mentioned if they are not living in the home. If they are living at home, files seldom comment on the quality, quantity, or frequency of their financial input. Clearly, these files are not about fathers, but about mothers and the responsibilities they are supposed to carry out (Swift, 1995a, pp. 104_105).
Until relatively recently, researchers have tended to have little comment on the tendency to mislabel "mothering" as "parenting" and to overlook the role of the father in cases of child neglect. One notable exception is research by Palacio-Quintin & Éthier (1993):
It is unacceptable for mothers to be held solely responsible for neglect and the only parent implicated in CPS investigations. Fathers have a direct influence on their children as well as indirect, in supporting the mother, emotionally and financially, in nurturing the child.... Neglectful parenting has to be seen in the context of the whole family, including the father. This relieves the mother of sole responsibility for the neglect but also to expand [sic] the available family resources. It is essential to convince workers of the fact that two parents are responsible for their children and focusing on the two increases the chance of successful intervention (pp. 157_161). Personalistic Causes: Mental/Psychological
The etiological research is far from clear about the individual characteristics of neglectful mothers.
Even though it is likely that reviewers of the relevant literature draw different conclusions regarding the role of personality and psychological resources more generally as a result of their varying theoretical orientations, they are certainly assisted by the inconsistency that is apparent in the database (Belsky, 1993, p. 417).
Despite these inconsistencies, many researchers have drawn conclusions about the personality characteristics of neglectful parents:
Neglectful parents are largely children themselves. Their infantile personalities seem to be largely the result of their own unmet childhood needs. They are isolated, have difficulty maintaining relationships, are verbally inaccessible, and lack the knowledge, judgement, and maturation [sic] to adequately parent their children. From their studies of neglectful mothers, Polansky and colleagues identified five types of personalities: the apathetic-futile, the impulse-ridden, the woman in reactive-depression, the mentally retarded, and the psychotic (Hanson, 1993, p. 120).
[M]altreating parents often are characterized by a lack of understanding of the emotional complexity of human relationships, especially
the parent_child relationship. They have difficulty seeing things
from the child's perspective or understanding behaviour in terms
of the child's developmental level and the context or situation.
Maltreating parents tend to think in global, all-or-nothing terms
rather than see the shades of grey that more realistically capture
human behaviour (Erickson & Egeland, 1996,
p. 13).
Compared to abusive and
non-abusive/non-neglectful parents, neglectful parents exhibit poor
problem-solving skills, intellectual deficits, and inappropriate
development expectations for their children (O. Barnett, 1997, p.
116).
Information-processing deficits among neglectful parents appear
to be
worthy of further research. As Toth underlined:
[T]he effect of poor quality caregiving and traumatic experiences on biological processes can provide important insight into the role of experience in altering the course of neurobiological growth (Cicchetti, 1993; Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994) (Toth, 1995, p. 561).
Crittenden (1993) looked at cognitive theory on information processing
and identified four states at which parents could fail to respond
to signs by their children. She theorized that parents might fail
to respond to stimuli indicative of children's need for care because
they "a) did not perceive the signal, b) interpreted the signal
as
not requiring a parental response,
c) knew that a response was needed but did not have a response available,
or d) selected a response but failed to implement it" (p. 27).
Crittenden also noted that each distinct failure to respond represents
a different type of neglect, associated with different types of
parental developmental history and each requiring different kinds
of interventions.
Greene et al. (1995) looked at two case studies involving mentally retarded parents with children placed in care due to abuse and neglect. They noted that "there is growing evidence that competence of parents with mental retardation (and developmental disability) can be improved
Additional research may be helpful in considering assessment and intervention strategies to facilitate such decisions. For example, perhaps the prospects of the parent assuming full-time parenting responsibility should be questioned very early, and different intervention strategies should be attempted if the incremental custody of the children is made contingent upon the parent's completion of child care tasks but does not effect sustained and generalized changes in child care practice (p. 433).
Belsky (1993) noted that more recent studies "seem more consistent in linking negative emotional states and traits with maltreatment and...the literature on depression (without regard to maltreatment) consistently highlights linkages between this state-trait and intrusive, hostile and rejecting care, as well as detached and unresponsive parenting" (p. 417).
One recent study looked at both psychological and social factors
of maltreating parents but did not find a link between neglect and
depression. Age, socio-economic status, social support, education,
household size, and gender, as well as psychiatric disorders including
substance abuse and depression, were examined. In order to overcome
the limitations
imposed by reliance on officially reported neglect cases that occurred
in the past, Chaffin et al. (1996) used data from the National Institute
for Mental Health's Epidemiologic Catchment Area survey. This study
followed 7,103 parents who did not self-report physical abuse or
neglect of their children in the first wave of the survey but who
self-reported physical abuse or neglect identified at Wave II. Physical
abuse and neglect were found to have distinct sets of risk factors,
with minimal overlap between the groups. Social and demographic
variables were found to be limited predictors of maltreatment, while
substance abuse disorders were strongly associated with the onset
of both abuse and neglect. Depression was found to be a strong risk
factor for physical abuse (Chaffin et al., 1996, p. 191).
Of the psychiatric disorders studied, substance abuse disorders
appear to be the most common and among the most powerfully associated
with maltreatment.... Depression was found to be more uniquely associated
with physical abuse rather than neglect once social factors and
substance abuse are statistically controlled.... This suggests that
the relationship between depression and
neglect may not be direct, as it appears to be for abuse, but may
be mediated by substance abuse, which is a common complication of
depression. The association of neglect with
These results contrast with a
Canadian study that found that "Mothers who neglect their children
tend to exhibit higher rates of depression compared to non-neglectful
mothers. Neglectful mothers also experience a high degree of stress"
(Éthier, Lacharité, & Couture, 1995) (Wiehe, 1996,
p. 50). Personalistic Causes: Substance Abuse
Research into parental drug use and neglect is very preliminary,
although it appears to be an area of growing research interest.
Research to date has been limited by vague definitions of "substance
abuse." Nonetheless, DiLeonardi hypothesized that children
of substance abusing mothers are more likely to be neglected than
abused and are more likely to suffer several subtypes of neglect
(NCCAN Chronic Neglect Symposium
proceedings, p. Appendix C_1).
Gaudin (1993a) also reported a link between substance abuse and neglect:
Abuse of alcohol or drugs is often present in cases of child neglect.
Recent reports from urban CPS [child protection services] agencies
indicate that substance abuse is a factor in a
growing percentage of child neglect cases. Estimates range from
a low of less than 24% [Martin and Walters, 1982] to 80 to 90% of
all child maltreatment reports [National Committee for the Prevention
of Child Abuse, 1989]. An earlier study found that 52% of the children
removed from their homes for severe child abuse or neglect had at
least one parent with a history of alcoholism [Famularo, 1986].
A study of women served in a Chicago alcoholism treatment program
reported that 65 to 75% of the women were neglectful toward their
children. The epidemic of cocaine addiction in urban inner-city
areas has resulted in large increases in the numbers of neglect
reports.... In spite of these associations, there is yet insufficient
data to conclude that substance abuse causes neglect, but it is
an increasingly significant contributing factor (p. 15). Personalistic
Causes: The Child
Newer models of child maltreatment make an effort to consider the dyadic nature of parenting and the role of parent_child interactions. Findings of these studies indicate:
Younger children appear more likely to experience maltreatment for a variety of reasons. One is that physical force is more often used against them (Straus, Gelles, & Steinmetz, 1980). Another is
There is not a great deal of research into the child's interactions
with the neglecting parent, although it would appear that there
is much to be learned in this area. (See, for example, recent findings
related to non-organic FTT syndrome, discussed in the section on
Effects.) The preliminary conclusions that have been drawn about
the role of the child in neglect can be summarized as
follows:
In summary, although I am inclined to draw the conclusion that parents play a larger role in the etiologic equation than do children with respect to the developmental-psychological context of maltreatment, there is no disputing the fact that children inadvertently contribute too (Belsky, 1993, p. 420).
Erickson & Egeland (1996) added to this hypothesis:
Although few would dispute that some children are more difficult
to care for than others, there is strong evidence from observational
studies that
child characteristics alone do not account for maltreatment. Research
taking a transactional view of parent_child
relationships demonstrates the power of parental sensitivity and
responsiveness in overcoming the child's difficulty (p. 14).
Using information collected by agencies receiving official reports of neglect, O. Barnett et al. (1997, p. 115) reported the following statistics:
The average age of neglected children is six years. Several sources indicate that the risk for neglect generally declines with age and the seriousness of injuries are more common for younger children.
According to the National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (NCCAN) in 1994, 51% of reported child neglect victims are under five years of age and 34% of those reports are for children under one year of age.
Few gender differences are associated with neglect. NCCAN indicated that 52% of reported cases were males and 48% were females.
Studies attempting to determine racial differences in rates of child neglect are fraught with methodological difficulties and, as a result, should be interpreted cautiously.
According to 1988 statistics, 63% of child neglect reports involved Caucasian children, 20% involved African-American children and 12% involved Hispanic children. Because census data indicate that 12.4%
A model of child maltreatment that appears to be waning in popularity
is the "cycle theory" of intergenera-tional transmission.
The theory, which was prominent in the 1960s and 1970s, is simple:
neglectful (and abusive) parents maltreat their children because
the parents themselves were neglected (abused) as children. "Closely
related to the `intergene-rational continuity of abuse' are theories
of maternal bonding and child development" (Swift, 1995a,
p. 96).
Psychological immaturity, characterized as "infantile personality,"
"impulse ridden," or "apathy-futility syndrome"
by Polansky, Chalmers, Williams, and Buttenwieser (1981) or lack
of "psychological complexity" by Pianta, Egeland, and
Erickson (1989) is a personality characteristic of
many neglectful mothers that is often related to their failure to
receive nurturing as children (Gaudin, 1993b, p. 69).
During the 1970s, two clinicians at the forefront of inquiry into the etiology and sequelae of child maltreatment observed that "the most constant fact [concerning child abusers] is that parents themselves were nearly always abused or battered or neglected as children" (Fontana, 1973, p. 74) and that "we see an unbroken line in the repetition of parental abuse from childhood into the adult years" (Steele, 1976, p. 15). More than 15 years after these comments were made, there are few in the scientific community who would embrace such remarks (Belsky, 1993, p. 415).
Massé explained that the cycle theory of child maltreatment is attractive and appeals to common sense but existing evidence of cause and effect is limited. A 1987 review by Kaufman and Zigler estimated that only 25% to 35% of victims of extreme physical abuse, sexual abuse, or neglect abuse their own children. Similarly, in 1989, Widom found 1.1% of adults abused as children abuse or neglect their children, compared with 1.0% of the control group. Massé suggested that the effects of placement on a child may be more serious (Massé, 1994).
O. Barnett et al. (1997) noted that few studies of the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment have looked specifically at child neglect and that there are contradictory results from the few that have. For example, a study by Éthier compared childhood histories of physically abusive mothers to neglectful mothers and found neglectful mothers more likely to have been victims of neglect, both physical and emotional. Conversely, a study by Zuravin and DiBlasio of teenage mothers found that neglectful mothers were no more likely to be abused or neglected than non-neglecting mothers but the neglecting mothers were more likely to have been sexually abused (p.129).
Swift questioned the ideological character of the cycle idea theory. The suggestion that mothers are unable to provide care primarily because they did not receive adequate care from their mothers supplies a satisfactory explanation for poor care, but "our attention is simultaneously drawn away from the social and economic context in which all these mothers have been doing their work" (Swift, 1995a, p. 99).
Another Canadian researcher shares Swift's concern. Massé noted that the cycle of violence theory masks the cycle of poverty and deprivation. He says that researchers should ask what risks are associated with the cycle of violence and maltreatment and what protective factors help parents to break the cycle of abuse (Massé, 1994, p. 248).
Belsky, however, pointed to "a few well-designed, prospective studies [that] clearly document a linkage between a reported history of childhood maltreatment and the perpetration of maltreatment." He hypothesized that certain maltreating individuals who report no history of maltreatment simply may not "recollect their troubled childhoods." It may be "that aggressive, antisocial behaviour is learned in childhood and [is] simply expressed in adulthood in the parenting role" (Belsky, 1993, p. 415). Further, parents' philosophy of discipline may be a factor in intergenerational transmission. It "seems plausible that abusive and neglectful childhoods may promote hostile personalities" (p. 415).
A study by Caliso and Milner found that women who broke the intergenerational cycle of transmission tended to be married to supportive and nurturant men. Belsky added that some studies suggest that physical attractiveness may be a mitigating factor in how children and women are treated. "Far more needs to be understood about who obtains the
The link between poverty and child neglect is clear. However, the meaning of this link is the cause of considerable debate. Data from the NIS-2 indicated that, of all subtypes of maltreatment, physical neglect is most clearly associated with poverty and Aid to Families with Dependent Children (AFDC) status (Erickson & Egeland, 1996, p. 14). Crittenden found that abusive parents have a higher level of revenue, social status and education than neglectful families (Palacio-Quintin & Éthier, 1993, p. 156).
In Canada, it has been estimated by child welfare agencies that between 66% and 75% of children in care come from poor families. One study of the cases served by a Toronto Children's Aid Society found 85% had incomes below the Statistics Canada low-income cut-off and a further 11% were economically vulnerable. At least half of the children in care come from single-parent families, yet single parents constitute about 13% of the families in Canada (Callahan, 1993, p. 182).
Rates of neglect are higher in
families characterized by very low income, unemployment, and
dependence on social assistance
(O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 115).
SES [socio-economic status], in fact, is a stronger predictor of child neglect than physical abuse.... In addition, approximately 51% of the children reported for neglect reside in single-female-headed households, and approximately 42% of the primary caretakers are unemployed (O. Barnett, 1997, p. 115).
Although child maltreatment permeates all socio-economic levels in our society, most maltreating parents are poor and welfare dependent. Simply stated, economic hardship and limited resources have long been linked with the occurrence of child abuse and neglect.... Nonetheless, it should be noted that the majority of families living below the poverty level provide adequate care to their children (D. Barnett et al., 1993, p. 15).
There are notable cases of neglect and abuse occurring in well-off families but there is reason to believe that the social and cultural resources of these families allow them to better hide their circumstances from child protection agencies. It is therefore possible that hidden neglect is equally distributed among social classes (Palacio-Quintin & Éthier, 1993, p. 156).
Callahan (1993) captured the link between neglect and poverty, as well as the potential to apply different meanings to this link:
One of the most troubling aspects of child welfare is this separation
between poverty and child care. The relationship between these two
factors is so self-evident it seems amazing that child welfare services
do not make it front and centre in their business. But they do not.
Instead, child welfare researchers and policy-makers have accepted
poverty as the context for the work and within that context have
set about to develop other responses. A recent
and widely acclaimed study illustrates this phenomenon (Polansky,
Gaudin, & Kilpatrick, 1992). A Maternal Characteristic Scale
[MCS] was applied to poor neglecting mothers and poor non-neglecting
mothers, almost half Afro-American. The scale was successful in
distinguishing between the two groups on their ability to relate,
their impulse control, their confidence, and their verbal accessibility.
Examples of such behaviour include "answers with single words,"
"hard to consider new ways," and "can laugh at herself."
The authors suggest that the scale can be used by social workers
to distinguish non-neglecting and neglecting mothers, and
conclude:
The MCS emphasizes unresolved schizoid elements and associated
problems with forming relationships, communicating, internalizing
controls, perceiving reality, self-observation, and empathy. At
the level of character traits, we speak of the Apathy-Futility Syndrome
and the Impulse-Ridden Character.... Review of specific behaviours
calls attention to rigidity, withdrawal, flatness of affect, and
lack of empathy
(pp. 278_279).
It would be equally possible to come to vastly different conclu
Dubowitz (1994) also commented on this point:
It is also possible that some professionals may harbour, wittingly
or unwittingly, a less sympathetic view toward the poor (Piven &
Cloward, 1971). Low-income families may be held largely accountable
for their circumstances and profes
sionals may be reluctant to become involved in financial issues.
This approach may also stem from professional blinders leading to
a narrow view of one's professional role (e.g., provide psychotherapy
to the mother). Pelton's (1978) classic paper on "the myth
of classlessness" describes the professional and political
interests served by falsely disconnecting child abuse and neglect
from poverty
(p. 558).
Dubowitz (1994) argued for a broader conceptual definition of neglect,
in which "poverty is clearly a major contributor, if not a
form of neglect
per se" (p. 557).
Of the different types of maltreatment, neglect is most strongly connected with poverty. One study found that the most severe neglect was among the poorest of the poor (Giovannoni & Billingsley, 1970). Indeed, many of the manifestations of physical neglect (e.g., inadequate clothing, exposure to environmental hazards, poor hygiene) may be primarily due to poverty (Dubowitz, 1994, p. 557).
Swift (1995a) also suggested that poverty is a form of neglect:
[I]t is also well established in discourse that neglect is a phenomenon of poor populations. Pelton (1981), for instance, has convincingly argued against the idea that maltreatment of
Swift (1995a) argued that child neglect "is a concept through
which more powerful groups maintain their
dominant position over particular vulnerable and marginalized groups"
(p. 34).
Categories of deviance such as neglect also work in more subtle ways. For instance, they produce a group of scapegoats, giving us somebody to blame when society is not working well. They also provide legitimation for designated authorities to enter into the private affairs of individuals and families (Swift, 1995a, p. 12).
Others question the cause and effect relationship:
Poverty has been linked by many researchers to neglect. The samples used, however, may have been biased because low-income, limited-resource families, during their quest for public assistance, tend to come into contact with maltreatment reporting agencies more frequently than families with higher levels of resources. What these studies may be doing is studying the behaviour of social welfare agencies involved in reporting child maltreatment cases. This may or may not have relevance for understanding the behaviour of the parents and children involved in neglectful situations (Albert & Barth, 1996) (Burke et al., 1998, p. 396).
More recent risk and causal models of child maltreatment have increasingly emphasized sociological factors that combine through multiple pathways to result in physical abuse or neglect (Chaffin et al., 1996, p. 191).
Major environmental factors (which are not specific to neglect but appear to be "robust" in regard to maltreatment in general) include:
violence in the marital
relationship,
parental unemployment,
general disorganization, and
the availability of a helpful, supportive social network, perhaps especially among single parents who lack intimate emotional support
(Erickson & Egeland, 1996,
p. 14).
Gaudin et al. (1993, p. 598) studied 102 neglectful families and
103 non-neglectful families, selected from US AFDC fund recipients.
"Neglect and control groups were composed of predominantly
low-income, single-parent, AFDC-recipient families; 60% of each
group were African-American, the rest were White (including one
Hispanic family in each group)." Their analysis of a wide range
of demographic factors found significant differences in only two
factors: the primary care providers in the neglectful families had
less
education (an average of grade 10 compared to grade 12 for the control
group) and more children (3.16 vs. 2.56). Environmental Causes:
Social Isolation
Social isolation is a factor that is increasingly linked to neglecting mothers. However, definitional problems also plague this construct and the meaning of the term seems to have changed over time.
In the 1960s, social isolation referred to a relatively narrow
concept: the state of being socially integrated or embedded within
the large community, based primarily on the number of contacts with
formal organizations. Its current usage, however, reflects a broad
set of findings taken from studies examining
a) the structural characteristics
of the parent's informal or formal network (i.e., number of contacts
with network members), b) the parent's perception that there is
adequate or available support, or c) whether the parent actually
received supportive resources in the past (Coohey, 1996, p. 243).
Jones (1996) noted that "[p]ractitioners appear to use the
concept to incorporate everything from the restricted network of
social contacts of an
impoverished, lonely, single parent through to social isolation
due to anti-social, hostile, argumentative behaviour, which has
led to relative
isolation" (p. 239).
A second difficulty in drawing conclusions about the relationship among components of the social isolation construct and child maltreatment can be attributed to differences between samples. "Inattention to socio-economic status or income is particularly problematic in the study of maltreaters and social networks, because they are over-represented in the lower classes or in communities with the highest poverty rates. Lower-income parents are likely to have lower-income members in their social networks which, in turn, is likely to affect the objective availability and flow of some resources to and from parents. Fewer actual resources, in turn, are likely to affect the perception that support is available or adequate. Moreover, lower income or socio-economic status has been found to be related to some structural properties, such as smaller networks; smaller networks tend to provide fewer resources, especially for women" (Coohey, 1996, p. 243).
Belsky maintained that social support has been linked with physical
and psychological well-being and has been conceptualized as a stress
buffer. "There is an abundance of evidence linking social isolation
and
limited social ties with elevated risk of child abuse and neglect"
(Belsky, 1993, p. 422).
Éthier, Palacio-Quintin,
Jourdan-Ionescu, Lacharité and Couture compared negligent
and violent mothers and found that when faced with difficulties,
negligent mothers used their personal supports (spouse, children,
parents, siblings) less than violent mothers (Palacio-Quintin &
Éthier, 1993, p. 158). Polansky found neglecting mothers
less involved in informal helping networks and described themselves
as more lonely (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 128).
Results of a study by Gaudin et al. (1993) "confirm and shed further light upon previously reported associations between neglectful parenting and the loneliness and social isolation of the parents. The significant correlations between self-reported loneliness and measures of social-network support confirm that neglectful parents not only report less support from neighbours, friends, and relatives but also feel more lonely and isolated" (p. 603).
Polansky has focused on psychosocial factors at the level of the individual and family. Across a series of studies, he and his colleagues have documented how isolation from social supports and extended family relationships plays a large role in the difficulties that neglecting parents have in
Social isolation and loneliness have been attributed by some to transience, although Belsky noted "the very real possibility that isolation and lack of social support is, at least in part, something that maltreating parents actively, even if inadvertently, contribute to, rather than something that simply happens to them" (Seagull, 1987) (Belsky, 1993, p. 422).
On the other hand, Coohey (1996), added:
Still we do not know why the structural properties of neglectful
mothers' networks differ in the first place.... One factor that
is often overlooked in treating child neglect is the neglectful
mother's educational background and IQ level. Both Polansky and
colleagues (1981) and Crittenden (1985) have found mental retardation
to be a
factor in child neglect; Crittenden found that 72% of the neglectful
mothers were retarded versus 5% of the physically abusive mothers
(p. 251).
To summarize, neglectful mothers had fewer members in their networks,
had fewer total contacts, had less contact with the members they
did have, perceived their members to be less supportive, and received
fewer instrumental and emotional resources from their network members
compared to mothers who did not abuse their children. Thus, the
neglectful mother's perception of support was consistent with the
actual receipt of fewer resources. If any particular type of maltreater
ought to be labelled "socially isolated," neglectful mothers
clearly had the most deficient social connections
(p. 250). Environmental Factors: Culture
Cultural attitudes and practices are seen by many to have an effect on child neglect and other forms of maltreatment.
In sum, then, although most child maltreatment takes place in the family and thus "behind closed doors," this immediate and even developmental context of maltreatment itself needs to be contextualized.
Despite the growing tendency to cite cultural issues as important to child welfare practice, there is little research in this area.
But as Garbarino and Ebata (1983) have observed, cultural and ethnic differences have received a treatment that might best be described as benign neglect. Moreover, serious problems reduce the confidence that can be placed in any conclusions that might be drawn from the few relevant studies (Belsky, 1993, p. 427).
[c]ollectively, these studies have consistently uncovered several problems associated with child neglect including social difficulties, intellectual deficits, emotional and behavioural problems, and physical consequences (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 116).
In reviewing studies investigating the effects of child neglect
on children's development, Crouch and Milner (1993) reported that
"studies exploring child neglect victim effects focus on child
`maltreatment' groups for which inclusion criteria tend to be broad"
(p. 50). Further, the lack of consensus on subtypes of neglect cause
difficulties in establishing definitions for research purposes;
the recruitment of subjects for studies often relies solely on social
service
agencies; samples are often small and poorly controlled; studies
tend to rely on retrospective self-reports or child protection reports;
and child neglect measures are often not standardized, so replication
of studies and the interpretation of results becomes difficult (p.
50).
Egeland and Sroufe, in a longitudinal study of four maltreatment groups of mothers, stated that a significantly higher proportion of neglected children were anxiously attached (e.g., overly dependent, clingy, prone to crying) at 12 and 18 months, compared to children in the control group (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 116).
Becker et al. (1995) summarized recent research into the effects
of neglect on children and reported that some studies have found
that neglected children display more behaviour problems than non-abused
children, such as apathy, passivity and less flexibility, persistence
and enthusiasm. They noted that
Hoffman-Plotkin and Twentyman found significant cognitive deficits
when comparing neglected and non-
neglected preschoolers and Wodarski found severe academic delays
among neglected children (pp. 29_30).
Hoffman-Plotkin and Twentyman (1984) reported that abused children were more aggressive than either neglected or nonmaltreated children, but the neglected children interacted less with peers than either abused or nonmaltreated children. Similarly, Crittenden (1985 and 1989) found that abused children were described as having difficult temperaments, became angry under stress and exhibited mild developmental delays. Neglected children, on the other hand, were passive, tended toward helplessness under stress, and showed significant development delays. In a review of studies from 1975 to 1992, Katz (1992) found that both abused and neglected children had language delays or disorders, but the problems of neglected children were more severe (p. 9).
A longitudinal study of developmental outcomes for physically abused
and neglected children revealed that physically neglected preschoolers
presented "the least positive and most negative affect"
of all types of maltreated children. Hoffman-Plotkin and Twentyman
found that neglected preschoolers engaged in the least number of
peer interactions when compared to physically abused and control
subjects. Allen and Oliver
found that neglect alone predicted both poor auditory comprehension
and poor verbal ability, even after controlling for the effects
of sex and socio-economic status (Becker et al., 1995, p. 30).
In one of the few studies involving adolescents, Henggeler, McKee, and Bourduin (1989) examined the relation between neglect and delinquency in 48 male adolescents divided into three groups: neglected-delinquent, delinquent, and control. Results indicated that delinquents from neglectful and nonneglectful families experience similar behavioural and family communication problems, suggesting that neglect itself was not a primary determinant. However, because the criterion for neglect was protective services involvement, it is possible that many of the nonneglectful families were in fact neglectful (Becker et al., 1995, p. 30).
Hanson (1993) reported that children who have been neglected demonstrate retarded growth, poor motor and language development, flat affect, indications of malnutrition, unattended medical problems, and an inability to conceptualize. Hanson also suggested the intergenerational transmission of neglect in that older children "often seek early emancipation and may begin the cycle all over again" (p. 120).
Many of these are young children left alone, dying in house fires. A follow-up study of different forms of maltreatment found the worse outcomes associated with neglect (Rivera & Widom, 1992). Several studies have identified other serious and long-term effects of child neglect, including failure to thrive, cognitive deficits, poor social skills, and increased criminal behaviour (Egeland, Sroufe, & Erickson, 1983; Fox, Lond, & Langlos, 1988; Rivera & Widom, 1992) (Dubowitz, 1994, pp. 556_557).
Neglected children, if they survive physically, often fail to develop
the confidence, concentration, and social skills that would enable
them to succeed in school and in relationships. The behaviour they
bring to the classroom sets them up for a continuing cycle of failure
and disappointment unless something happens to make a difference.
Even the most subtle kinds of emotional neglect have a dramatic
effect on children's development, especially during the early years
of life.... [A]ttachment theory provides a useful framework for
understanding the impact of neglect. This theory proposes that the
infant's relationships with primary caregivers are the prototypes
for subsequent relationships.... For example, the child whose mother
fails to respond to his or her signals will eventually shut down,
no longer seeking or accepting contact with her.... Then, when the
child enters the new social world of school, those old expectancies
and behaviours continue to play out in regard to learning, peer
relationships, and response to teachers (Erickson & Egeland,
1996, p. 15).
The Minnesota Mother-Child Project is a longitudinal study that was designed to follow the development of a sample of 267 children born to first-time mothers identified as being at risk for parenting problems due to poverty, youth, low education, lack of support, and unstable life circumstances. Findings led Erickson and Egeland (1996) to conclude that emotional neglect seems to be the most serious form of maltreatment, in terms of the consequences.
In many ways, our study shows the consequences of emotional neglect (or what we call psychologically unavailable parenting) to be even more profound than physical neglect and the other types of maltreatment. Nearly all of the children in this group were anxiously attached, with the majority of those classified as anxious-avoidant. In each of the
Although the effectiveness of intervention with neglecting families
has not been studied adequately, limited evidence suggests that
interventions are successful with no more than 50% of families.
The most effective interventions are comprehensive and relatively
long term (Gaudin, 1993) (Erickson & Egeland, 1996,
p. 16).
Cohn and Daro also warned of
disappointing results.
Reviews of clinical intervention programs and federally funded demonstration projects reflect relatively poor outcomes (e.g., 30% success rate) and a high reoccurrence of maltreating behaviours (e.g., 66%) in neglectful parents and caregivers. These authors suggested that positive findings were associated with skill-training groups (i.e., home management and social skills focus), parent education and support groups, family counselling, home-based counselling to remediate daily living skills deficits, and the use of lay counsellors. In contrast, programs lasting less than 6 months, with more traditional emphasis on parent-focused interventions and casework, were associated with the poorest outcomes (Becker et al., 1995, p. 37).
Daro (1988) reviewed 19 NCCAN [National Center on Child Abuse and
Neglect]
demonstration programs from 1978 to 1982 and found that "in
only 53% of the neglectful families was there improvement in the
family's overall level of functioning, and 70% were judged likely
to recidivate after
Working with neglectful families appears to be a difficult job. Research by Bath indicated that those who have already neglected are one of the most difficult populations to reach through current service models (Rose & Meezan, 1993, p. 287). Some authors attribute this to the nature of the client population:
A major difficulty in serving [chronically neglectful] families
is the emotionally draining effect that the apathy of neglectful
families may have on professionals. The hopelessness and helplessness
of these families make it extremely difficult for workers to initiate
and follow through on plans that might alleviate the families' situation
(DiLeonardi, 1993,
p. 559).
To date, most clinical interventions have focused on treating the neglecting mother, rather than the neglected child.
As is typical of the abuse and neglect literature, the empirical studies that are available (e.g., Dawson, de Armas, McGrath & Kelly, 1986; Gaudin, Wodarski, Arkinson & Avery, 1991; Lutzker, 1990; Lutzker & Newman, 1986; Lutzker & Rice, 1984) have focused the major intervention on parents rather than on children.... The majority of the clinical and empirical information on treatment programs for neglected children focuses solely on infants and preschool-age children (Becker et al., 1995, p. 30).
Swift (1995a) viewed this focus in the following terms:
It is important to notice that while the children's needs warrant our entry into the private home, it is the needs of the mother rather than the children that become the focus of intervention. The role of the state is to produce and enforce care for children through the family, which usually means through the mother. It is the need for a change in mother that provides the justification for intervention, and it further explains the kinds of interventions that child welfare systems typically offer. Mothers are presented as having deficit needs; they have not themselves been nurtured adequately. This explanation is what Fraser describes as "needs interpretation," a function
The few programs directed at
children have tended to provide
therapeutic day care, which appears
to be effective in addressing some of the deficits associated with
neglect.
[M]ost of the treatment programs available to address the needs of neglected children involve therapeutic day-care programs (e.g., Culp et al., 1987; Culp et al., 1991).... Overall, therapeutic day-care programs have resulted in significant developmental gains in preschoolers. However, limited information on long-term treatment success is available, and adaptations of this approach for older children is non-existent (Becker et al., 1995, p. 30).
Becker et al. (1995) concluded that more empirical studies on the
treatment of neglected children are
needed and that "[f]uture research should address the needs
of older children and adolescents, utilize
standardized measures, separate neglect from other forms of abuse,
and measure outcome through recidivism data as well as the children's
long-
term academic performance and psychological adjustment" (p.
30).
Early interventions are seen to be important:
Because neglect is particularly damaging in infancy, it is important to work with families as early in the infant's life as possibleor, preferably, even before the baby is born (Erickson & Egeland, 1996, p. 16).
Multiple interventions are also seen as important:
The best news, in fact, is that because of the "discovery" that child maltreatment is multiply determined, no "magic bullet" must be identified and targeted before intervention efforts can be initiated.... Although the multidetermined nature of child maltreatment suggests that there are many targets to focus prevention and remediation efforts, it simultaneously alerts psychologists to the fact that directing efforts at any single target is not likely to be particularly successful (Belsky, 1993, p. 428).
Coohey (1996) also recommended a variety of interventions, beginning with the following:
[T]he premise that personal social networks are the major avenue by which parents receive most of the resources that were included in this study. Thus, it is not reasonable to assume that individual practitioners can provide a permanent or adequate source of, for example, emotional support to
Evaluation studies of multi-service interventions have demonstrated some positive results.
The NCCAN, for example, has recently funded a series of multi-service projects directed at chronically neglectful families. Evaluations of these projects have indicated that a combination of parenting groups, intensive in-home counselling, and supportive interventions (e.g., parapro-fessional aides) has been effective in improving neglectful parenting practices.... Two recent studies suggest, however, that outcomes for neglecting families are less positive than for abusive families or families of delinquents (O. Barnett et al., 1997, p. 131).
Dubowitz et al. (1993) echoed the call for varied interventions tailored to the individual situation (p. 10). Cicchetti and Toth (1995) emphasized developmental appropriateness:
In examining the needs of maltreating families, it becomes clear that the integration and coordination of services are critical if intervention is to be effective. The more unified and comprehensive the interventions that are available within an individual treatment centre, the greater the likelihood that splintered services will be avoided.... Most basically, the survival needs of the family for food, clothing, and shelter must be met before the family members can be engaged in more complex psychological and behavioural change. The clinician working with the family must be responsive to establishing a trusting relationship with both parents and children. Clinical interventions need to be sensitive to critical stage-salient issues of the child, and interventions with parents and children should focus on those issues.... Intervention with maltreating families also must be sensitive to variations in family organization, structure, roles, and patterns of relating that are influenced by cultural, racial, and ethnic differences (p. 555).
Interventions require clear treatment goals:
To formulate appropriate interventions with neglectful families it is critical to distinguish between inadequate supervision that is related to the parent's
Some authors have recognized the importance of preventive strategies.
Nevertheless, because the demographic data clearly indicate that poverty and early and extensive childbearing provide fertile soil in which child maltreatment can grow, it is difficult to imagine that major strides can be made in the battle to prevent, much less remediate, child maltreatment so long as impoverished women, particularly those who are young, are rearing multiple and closely spaced offspring on their own, without sufficient social supports, or both. This observation suggests that fertility planning, education, employment, and economic assistance will be required.... (Belsky, 1993, p. 428).
Palacio-Quintin and Éthier (1993) suggested economic and social policies to address child neglect in Canada, such as reducing poverty, improving housing, job opportunities and early intervention. New psychosocial models of early intervention are needed (p. 162).
Tracy et al. (1993) noted that:
The three major family stress factors that affect families ser-viced by child welfare agencies in this study were substance abuse, economic difficulties, and poor living conditions. These factors represent large social-environmental problems that are generally outside the control or auspice of the child welfare system. This finding is significant in light of the fact that the major thrust of service delivery was counselling and therapeutic services (p. 26).
Belsky (1993) also acknowledged the importance of income supports and improved housing for poor families.
As stated repeatedly, poverty is a major contributing factor to child maltreatment. Thus it seems likely that guaranteed minimal incomes, child allowances, and housing benefits would reduce the risk of maltreatment (p. 429).
Gaudin (1993b) proposed a number of elements for designing interventions, such as mobilizing concrete formal
Broadening the focus of treatment is an area that appears promising. Multi-service interventions and those that included all family members, rather than focused on the principal care provider, were more successful with neglectful families, according to Daro (Gaudin, 1993b, p. 77). Group methods also appear successful, as do intensive, weekly, in-home casework counselling focusing on concrete problem solving.
Researchers also note that "empowerment" can be effective
in helping neglectful families, although the term appears to be
open to some interpretation. Empowerment can be conceptualized as
a philosophy, as a paradigm, as a process, as a partnership, as
a performance, and as a percep
tion, according to Landsman (NCCAN, 1997b, p. 18). Empowerment-based
practice entails
exchanges between clients and professionals, or between help-seekers and help-givers;
partnership and mutual respect among all parties involved;
a proactive or strength-based stance toward individuals and families based on the assumption that people are capable of acting competently and of enhancing their competence; and
a cognitive componentto gain a sense of self-efficacy, clients must attribute changes to their own activities or actions.
Several authors have suggested that life-skills training has been effective.
Project 12-Ways is one of the most carefully documented and successful programs for neglectful parents reported in the research literature. The program uses in-home behavioural training to teach neglectful parents grocery-shopping and menu-planning skills, skills to remedy specific safety hazards and improve the cleanliness in the home, and identification of children's illness symptoms. Parents and children were taught specific skills using the
Interventions providing direct service to children appear to help remedy some of the effects of neglect, according to Daro, but there is little reliable empirical evidence of this. Her review indicated that therapeutic child care programs providing cognitive stimulation, cultural enrichment, and motor and social skill development have a significant impact on the child's ability to function (Gaudin, 1993b, pp. 83_84).
NCCAN, a part of the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services,
funded six demonstration projects to help neglectful families in
1988. Family empowerment, group work, and paraprofessionals or volunteers
were used to some extent by all of the projects. All of the families
served had an income lower than the poverty level. The Childhood
Level of Living (CLL) Scale was used to assess families before and
after
intake. Services lasted an average of 18 months (range three months
to over two years).
Many families showed improvement in the areas of family socialization
or activities, household cleanliness, and appropriate child discipline
(p. 561). Overall the project families scored at a mean of 64% of
the CLL norms for minimum adequate parenting, which increased to
an average of 82% minimum adequacy at the end (DeLeonardi &
Johnson, 1993) (DiLeonardi, 1993, pp. 557_562).
Neglect is often embedded in a larger pattern of dysfunction and, in many cases, environmental chaos, making it difficult or impossible to separate the impact of neglect from other environmental influences.... [I]ntervention efforts most likely will need to address the entire matrix of home and family variables that support or impede children's development (Erickson & Egeland, 1996, p. 10).
As Swift (1995a) has indicated, the usual focus of child welfare personnel is, by legal necessity, on the culpability of the parent.
One reason neglect files are so distressing to read is that we
see in them the dreadful conditions children endure during the lengthy
period the state requires to establish evidence against their parents.
Children are condemned to live like this not only because their
parents are unable or unwilling to do better, but also because the
only helping tool society has provided itself is to find parents
guilty. While scholars debate the desirable definitional breadth
of neglect, the actual standard of care enforced through the present
system is desperately lowsurely well
below any minimum standard scholars would care to commit to paper
(p. 87).
A conceptual shift is required, according to some authors. In light of what she calls "a hundred years of failure to either save children or change mothers," Swift recommended a radical rethinking of the child welfare system, with many shifts in funding, organization, and orientation so that service providers can become concerned with the welfare of children rather than with protective practices. She welcomed fellow Canadian Marilyn Callahan's "recentand courageoussuggestion that neglect be eliminated as a child welfare category" altogether.
The ideological baggage neglect carries with it, the overly legalized system developed to make determinations of neglect, the paltry resources associated with it, and the abject failure of its use in improving life for clientschildren and parents alikeall argue for a radical change in direction. Workers who now spend much of their time policing families might actually be able to provide service, were resources to be
Indeed, Callahan's "courageous" suggestion appears to make good sense:
In [separating child apprehension from voluntary services to families],
several models could be explored. In any model, the so-called crime
of neglect should simply disappear from the child welfare statutes.
Instead, child welfare statutes could be reframed to define the
caring services to be provided and the circumstances under which
they will be provided. If chronic neglect is primarily a matter
of poverty, frequently the poverty of disadvantaged women, then
it should be dealt with as a resource issue rather than a personal,
individual problem. If situational neglect occurs, such as the abandonment
of children, then such problems can be dealt with by providing care
and resources to children, locating parents, and helping them make
plans for their children. Proving them unfit to care for their children
in either case is irrelevant, as it wastes court time and damages
parent_child relationships. Voluntary care orders would remain.
In any event, neglect could remain within the Criminal Code for
those difficult cases where serious neglect occurs yet help is refused
(Callahan, 1993, p. 205).
Other authors have come to similar conclusions. One important shift in focus appears to be a need to start addressing child neglect prevention:
In our opinion, one of the major directions for both practice and research in the area of child neglect is the implementation and careful evaluation of programs designed to prevent neglect.... We concur with Aber and his colleagues that programs must be designed, targeted, and evaluated within a clearly articulated theory on the development of maltreated children and the factors that lead to and perpetuate maltreatment. We believe that attachment theory provides a good place to begin (Erickson & Egeland, 1996, p. 16).
For those who take a less radical approach to change, a number of recommendations have been made to improve the research base. Cicchetti and Toth's (1995) recommendations include the following:
consider definitional issues,
expand cultural and ethnic sensitivity,
elucidate the development processes contributing to adaptation and maladaptation,
conduct research on the whole family,
address the effects of
co-occurring risk factors,
measure the psychological and biological correlates of functioning in maltreated children,
further articulate the links between child maltreatment and psychopathology,
expand public educational efforts, and
disseminate knowledge to legislators and policy
advocates.
The current research points to the seriousness of the effects of neglect on children and the limits and deficiencies of prevention and treatment efforts. As Douglas Barnett et al. (1993) summarized:
[S]cientists have been successful in increasing our knowledge of child maltreatment, but our nation has been comparatively unsuccessful in benefiting from this progress (p. 44).
Child neglect's strong link with poverty cannot be overlooked.
Hewlett's (1993) examination of child neglect in wealthy, industrialized
nations points to a number of broad economic and political factors
that affect child neglect. Her analysis suggested that child neglect
is not a phenomenon that will disappear on its own in the near future.
Hewlett points to shrinking wages in Anglo-American economies since
the mid-1970s, the increased number of two-income families, longer
work weeks, stresses on parents, the high number of absentee fathers
(in the United States, one quarter of children under 18 are growing
up without fathers10 million as a result of divorce and 5
million as a result of out-of-wedlock births), and low support payments
to mothers
(pp. 5_13).
One question that researchers in Canada must continue to ask is how relevant American child neglect research is to the Canadian situation. Does Canada, with lower levels of abject family poverty than the United States, have a lighter societal burden related to child neglect? Do our social programs mitigate the occurrence of child neglect compared with our neighbours to the south? Does the American crack cocaine epidemic foreshadow increasing drug abuse in Canada?
Trocmé et al.'s (1994) research concerning child maltreatment investigations in Ontario suggested that neglect does represent a lower proportion of child maltreatment cases, compared with US statistics. Maltreatment investigations conducted in Ontario in 1993 (46,683) showed that
19,352 (41.4%) involved suspected physical abuse,
11,846 (25.3%) involved suspected sexual abuse,
4,727 (10%) involved suspected emotional maltreatment.
Maltreatment was substantiated in 27% of these cases, suspected in 30% and unfounded in 42% (p. iii).
In comparison, the US NIS-3 (1993) found a total of 2,815,600 reported maltreatment cases of which
614,100 (21.8%) were cases of physical abuse,
300,200 (10.6%) were cases of sexual abuse,
532,200 (18.9%) were cases of emotional abuse,
1,335,100 (47.4%) were cases of physical neglect, and
585,100 (20.7%) were cases of emotional neglect.
Canadian programs and interventions may also have a mitigating
factor on child neglect. A number of longitudinal studies currently
under way (e.g., Better Beginnings, Better Futures) may shed
some light on child neglect research and interventions in this country.
Other programs that may have an effect include the
Community Action Program for Children (CAPC) initiatives. These
are community-based programs funded by Health Canada that are designed
to improve the health and well-being of children six years of age
or younger, as well as their families, who are in difficult situa
tions. The ongoing evaluation of the effects of these programs may
also increase the knowledge base concerning effective interventions.
Other Canadian programs that should be examined in light of child
neglect research include the various Headstart programs across the
country, Babies' Best Start in Ontario, home visiting programs,
and enriched, early childhood programs.
Diverse research from a variety of disciplines and fields continues to broaden our understanding of child maltreatment issues. There is new information about infant brain development that should be examined for relevance with regard to prevention for children. As each new piece of information about social and human development helps to fill in the pieces of some long-standing puzzles, they also result in new puzzles and concepts that help us to question assumptions and re-visit existing models and theories.
Broad, basic questions posed by Swift and other researchers have not yet been addressed in a systematic way across Canada. Some of these questions relate to the basic direction of child welfare work in Canada. There is much policy and research work yet to be done regarding child neglect in particular and child welfare in general, beyond the current emphasis on child death reviews and the narrower focus on "child safety."
It is important that we continue to question the historical biases, social ideologies, and political expediency
File Number:
Worker's Name: Date :
The two factors to be considered in assessing level of supervision are avoidability (i.e., extent to which a caretaker can be expected to anticipate and prevent) and severity of harm, or potential harm. Three specific types of harm that may result from failure to supervise: physical harm, sexual molestation, criminal activity/child under 13.
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Adequateprovisions made to ensure child's safety; caretaker knows child's whereabouts and activities; clear limits set on activities.
2. Inconsistentchild is occasionally exposed to situation that could cause moderate harm (e.g., young school-aged child occasionally left alone, parents do not monitor whereabouts of adolescent who occasionally comes home late in the evening).
3. Inadequatechild is often exposed to situations that could cause moderate harm, or there is a slight possibility that the child could suffer serious harm (e.g., young school-aged child often left unsupervised, or infant occasionally left alone while sleeping).
4. Seriously Inadequatechild is often exposed to situations that could cause serious harm (e.g., abandonment, home used as "crack house" and drugs left within reach of child, child often left to wander in dangerous neighbourhood, toddler often exposed to hazardous situations).
s/o
Physical harm or substantial risk of physical harm due to the caretaker's failure to care and provide for the child adequately.
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Regular and nutritional meals provided.
2. Meals irregular and often not prepared, but child's functioning is not impared.
3. Meals irregular and often not prepared; child's functioning is impaired (e.g., child is hungry and has difficulty concentrating in class).
4. Inadequate food providedthere is a substantial risk that the child will suffer from malnutrition (e.g., infant given diluted formula).
5. Child displays clinical symptoms of malnutrition; medical attention and/or rehabilitative diet required (e.g., weight loss, anemia, dehydration, etc.).
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Child is clean and adequately clothed.
2. Inadequate clothing or hygiene, but this does not appear to affect child's functioning.
3. Inadequate clothing or hygiene limits child's functioning (e.g., unable to go outdoors because of lack of clothing, isolated by peers because of hygiene or appearance).
4. Inadequate clothing or hygiene likely to cause illness requiring medical treatment (e.g., infestation of head lice).
5. Illness requiring medical treatment due to inadequate clothing
or hygiene (e.g., serious infection due to poor diaper care, intestinal
disorder).
s/o
"Treatment not provided" includes refusing or being unavailable or unable to consent to treatment. The extent to which harm could be avoided should be considered in terms of three factors: (a) whether a reasonable layman would recognize that a problem needs professional attention; or (b) whether a professional has recommended services or treatment; or (c) availability and/or effectiveness of treatment or services (e.g., the questionable effectiveness of services for chronic teen manners).
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Basic medical care provided.
2. Preventive medical care not provided (e.g., no regular checkups).
3. Medical care not provided for injury or illness causing avoidable distress.
4. Medical care not provided for injury or illness causing avoidable distress and interfering with child's functioning (e.g., chronic absence from school due to untreated illness).
5. Medical care not provided for injury or illness, which could lead to permanent impairment or death (e.g., infant vomiting or diarrhea leading to dehydration).
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Parents anticipate and respond to child's emotional needs.
2. Inconsistent response to emotional distress (e.g., responds only to crisis situations).
3. Services or treatment not provided in response to emotional distress; child at substantial risk of severe emotional or behavioural problems (anxiety, depression, withdrawal, self-destructive or aggressive behaviour, child under 13 engaging in criminal activity).
4. Services or treatment not provided in response to emotional
distress, child experiencing severe emotional or behavioural
problems.
s/o
Unknown/Does Not Apply
1. Child's developmental and educational needs are met.
2. Child's developmental and educational needs are inconsistently met (e.g., limited infant stimulation, child could benefit from remedial help in one or two subjects, child having academic difficulties due to poor school attendance).
3. Services or treatment are not provided in response to identified learning or developmental problems (e.g., learning disability diagnosed but caretakers refuse remedial help).
4. Child has suffered or will suffer serious/permanent delay due to inattention to developmental/educational needs (e.g., Non-organic Failure to Thrive identified but caretakers refuse remedial help).
For further information contact Nico Trocmé (416_978_5718; nico.trocme@utoronto.ca), Bell Canada Child Welfare Research Unit, Faculty of Social Work, University of Toronto.
s/o
American Association for Protecting Children
AFDC
Aid to Families with Dependent Children (USA)
CAPC
Community Action Program for Children
CLL
Childhood Level of Living
CNI
Child Neglect Index (Ontario)
CPS
Child Protection Services
CWB
Child Well-Being
DHHS
Department of Health and Human Services (USA)
FTT
Failure to Thrive
MCS
Maternal Characteristic Scale
NCCAN
National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect (USA)
NIS
National Incidence Study for National Incidence and Prevalence of Child Abuse and Neglect Study (USA). These studies are conducted periodically and are differentiated by NIS_1, NIS_2, NIS_3, etc.
NOFT
Non-organic Failure to Thrive (Also referred to as NFTT)
SES
Socio-economic Status (USA)
Barnett, Douglas; J. I. Vondra; S. M. Shonk. "Self-Perceptions,
Motivation, and School Functioning of Low-Income Maltreated and
Comparison Children." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(5)
(1996): 397_410.
Barnett, Douglas. J. T. Manly;
D. Cicchetti. "Defining Child
Maltreatment: The Interface Between Policy and Research." In
D. Cicchetti and S. L. Toth (eds.), Child Abuse, Child Development,
and Social Policy, 7_73. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex Publishing, 1993.
Barnett, Ola; C. L. Miller-Perrin; R. D. Perrin. Family Violence Across the Lifespan. New York: Sage, 1997.
Bath, Howard; D. A. Haapala.
"Intensive Family Preservation Services with Abused and Neglected
Children: An Examination of Group Differences." Child Abuse
and Neglect 17(2) (1993): 213_225.
Bath, Howard; D. A. Haapala.
"Family Preservation Services: What Does the Outcome Research
Really Tell Us?" Social Services Review 68 (1994): 386_404.
Becker, Judith; J. L. Alpert;
D. S. BigFoot; B. L. Bonner et al. "Empirical Research on Child
Abuse Treatment: Report by the Child Abuse and Neglect Treatment
Working Group, American
Psychological Association." Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 24 (Suppl.) (1995): 23_46.
Belsky, Jay. "Etiology of Child Maltreatment: A Developmental-Ecological Analysis." In Psychological Bulletin 114(3) (1993): 413_434.
Berry, Marianne. "The Relative Effectiveness of Family Preservation
Services with Neglectful Families." In E. S. Morton and R.
K. Grigsby (eds.), Advancing Family
Preservation Practice, 70_98. Newbury Park, Calif.: Sage, 1993.
Burke, Judith; J. Chandy;
A. Dannerbeck; J. W. Watt. "The Parental Environment Cluster
Model of Child Neglect: An Integrative Conceptual Model." Child
Welfare 77 (1998): 389_405.
Callahan, Marilyn. "Feminist
Approaches: Women Recreate Child Welfare." In B. Wharf (ed.),
Rethinking Child Welfare in Canada.
Toronto: McClelland and Stewart, 1993.
Crouch, Julie L.; J. S. Milner.
"Effects of Child Neglect on
Children." Criminal Justice and Behaviour 20(1) (1993):
45_65.
DiLeonardi, J. "Families in Poverty and Chronic Neglect of Children." Families in Society 74(9) (1993): 557_562.
Downs, Susan; L. B. Costin;
E. J. McFadden. "Protecting Children from Neglect and Abuse."
In Child Welfare and Family Services Policy and Practice,
p. 182. New York: Longman, 1996.
Drake, Brett; S. Pandey.
"Understanding the Relationship
Between Neighborhood Poverty and Specific Types of Child Maltreatment." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(11) (1996): 1003_1018.
Dubowitz, H.; A. Klockner; R. H. Starr; M. M. Black. "Community
and
Professional Definitions of Child Neglect." Child Maltreatment 3(3) (1998): 235_243.
Dubowitz, Howard. "Neglecting the Neglect of Neglect." Journal of
Interpersonal Violence 9(4) (1994): 556_560.
Dubowitz, Howard; M. Black.
"Medical Neglect." In J. Briere et al. (eds.), The
APSAC Handbook on Child Maltreatment, 227_241. Thousand Oaks,
Calif.: Sage, 1994.
Dubowitz, Howard; M. Black;
R. H. Starr; S. Zuravin. "A Conceptual Definition of Child
Neglect." Criminal Justice and Behaviour 20(1) (1993):
8_26.
Chaffin, M.; K. Kelleher;
J. Hollenberg. "Onset of Physical Abuse and Neglect: Psychiatric,
Substance Abuse, and Social Risk Factors from Prospective Community
Data." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(3) (1996): 191_203.
Cicchetti, D.; S. L. Toth. "A
Developmental Psychopathology Perspective on Child Abuse and Neglect." Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34(5) (1995): 541_565.
Cicchetti, D.; S. L. Toth;
K. Hennessy. "Child Maltreatment and School Adaptation: Problems
and Promises." In D. Cicchetti and S. L. Toth (eds.), Child
Abuse, Child Development, and Social Policy 301_330. Norwood,
N.J.: Ablex, 1993.
Coohey, Carol. "Neglectful Mothers, Their Mothers, and Partners: The Significance of Mutual Aid." Child Abuse and Neglect 19(8) (1994): 885_895.
Coohey, Carol. "Child Maltreatment: Testing the Social Isolation
Hypothesis." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(3) (1996): 241_254.
Crittenden, Patricia M. "An
Information-Processing Perspective on the Behaviour of Neglectful
Parents." Criminal Justice and Behaviour 20(1) (1993):
27_48.
Erickson, Martha; B. Egeland.
"Child Neglect." In J. Briere et al. (eds.), The APSAC
Handbook on Child Maltreatment, 4_20.
Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage, 1996.
Éthier, Louise S.; C. Biron; P. Pinard et al. "Réussir en négligence." Le Groupe de recherche en développement de l'enfant et de la famille (Gredef), n.p. Trois-Rivières, 1998.
Federal-Provincial Working Group on Child and Family Services
Information. Child Welfare in Canada. Ottawa: National Clearinghouse
on Family Violence, 1994.
Gable, Sara. "School-Age and
Adolescent Children's Perceptions of Family Functioning in Neglectful
and Non-neglectful Families." Child Abuse and Neglect 22(9)
(1997): 859_867.
Gaudin, James Jr. Child Neglect: A Guide for Intervention. Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1993a.
Gaudin, James M. "Effective
Intervention with Neglectful
Families." Criminal Justice and Behaviour 20(1) (1993b):
66_89.
Gaudin, James M.; N. A. Polansky; A. C. Kilpatrick; P. Shilton.
"Loneliness, Depression, Stress, and Social Supports in Neglectful
Families." American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 63(4)
(1993):
597_605.
Gaudin, James M.; N. A. Polansky; A. C. Kilpatrick; P. Shilton. "Family Functioning in Neglectful Families." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(4) (1996): 363_377.
Gauthier, L.; G. Stollak; L. Messé;
J. Aronoff. "Recall of Childhood Neglect and Physical Abuse
as Differential Predictors of Current Psychological Functioning." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(7) (1996): 549_559.
Giardino, Angelo P. "Introduction to Abuse and Neglect."
In Child
Physical Abuse and Neglect, 1_20. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage,
1997.
Giovannoni, Jeanne M. "A Child Centered Resolution." In Chronic Neglect Symposium Proceedings, 1993, 8_10. National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1993.
Goldson, Edward. "Children with Disabilities and Child Maltreatment." Child Abuse and Neglect 22(7) (1998): 663_667.
Haecker, T. A.; M. M. Cockerill. 1997. "Neglect and Failure to Thrive." In Child Physical Abuse and Neglect, 169_194. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage, 1997.
Hanson, Karen. "The Neglect of Children." In Understanding
Child Abuse and Neglect, 95_122. Needham Heights, Mass.: Allyn
and Bacon, 1993.
Harrington, Donna; H. Dubowitz;
M. M. Black; A. Binder.
"Maternal Substance Use and
Neglectful Parenting: Relations with Children's Development." Journal of Clinical Child Psychology 24(3) (1995): 258_263.
Hawley, Theresa L.; T. G. Halle;
R. E. Drasin; N. G. Thomas.
"Children of Addicted Mothers."
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 65(3) (1995): 364_379.
Hébert, J.; C. Chamberland. "Une expérience
de prévention sociale qui remet en question nos services
enfance-famille." Nouvelles pratiques sociales 6(2)
(1995):
59_69.
Hewlett, Sylvia Ann. Child Neglect in Rich Nations. New York: Unicef, 1993.
Iwaniec, Dorota. The Emotionally Abused and Neglected Child.
Chichester, Eng.: Wiley, 1995.
Iwaniec, Dorota. "Evaluating Parent Training for Emotionally Abusive and Neglectful Parents: Comparing Individual Versus Individual and Group Intervention." Research on Social Work Practice 7(3) (1997): 329_349.
Jones, David P. H. "Parental Social Isolation (editorial)." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(3) (1996): 239_240.
Kendall-Tackett, K. A.; J. Eckenrode. "The Effects of Neglect
on Academic Achievement and Disciplinary
Problems: Developmental
Perspective." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(3) (1996):
161_169.
Kotch, Jonathan B.; D. C. Browne; C. L. Ringwalt; P. W. Steward.
"Risk of Child Abuse or Neglect in a
Cohort of Low-Income Children." Child Abuse and Neglect 19(9) (1995): 1115_1130.
Kotch, Jonathan B.; D. C. Browne; C. L. Ringwalt et al. "Stress,
Social Support, and Substantiated
Maltreatment in the Second and Third Years of Life." Child
Abuse and Neglect 21(11) (1997): 1025_1037.
Leventhal, J. M. "Twenty Years Later: We do Know How to Prevent Child Abuse and Neglect." Child Abuse and Neglect 20(8) (1996): 647_653.
MacMillan, Harriet L.;
J. H. MacMillan; D. R. Offord et al. "Primary Prevention of
Child
Physical Abuse and Neglect: A Critical Review. Part I." Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35(5) (1997): 835_856.
Massé, Raymond. "Antécédents de violence et transmission intergénérationelle de la maltraitance." P.R.I.S.M.E. 4(2_3) (1994): 239_250.
Massé, Raymond; M-F. Bastien. "Isolement social, pauvreté et négligence : une étude cas-témoin." In Rapport de recherche. Direction de la santé publique, Équipe écologie humaine et sociale: Montréal, 1995.
Massé, Raymond; M-F. Bastien. "La pauvreté génère-t-elle
la maltraitance? Espace de pauvreté et misère sociale
chez deux échantillons de mères défavorisées." Revue québécoise de psychologie (1996):
3_24.
Meston, John. Child Abuse
and Neglect Prevention Programs. Ottawa: Vanier Institute of
the
Family, p. 146, 1993.
Minty, Brian; Pattinson, Graham. "The Nature of Child Neglect." British Journal of Social Work 24 (1994): 733_747.
National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Child Maltreatment Issues 1994 Research Review: Volume I. Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996.
National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Child Maltreatment Issues 1995 Research Review: Volume I. Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1997a.
National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Chronic Neglect Symposium Proceedings, 1993. Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1997b.
National Research Council. Understanding Child Abuse and Neglect. Washington: National Academy Press, 1993.
Nelson, Kristine E.; E. J. Saunders; M. J. Landsman. "Chronic Child Neglect in Perspective." Social Work 38(6) (1993): 661_671.
Ney, Philip G.; T. Fung;
A. R. Wickett. "The Worst
Combinations of Child Abuse and Neglect." Child Abuse and
Neglect 18(9) (1994): 705_714.
Palacio-Quintin, Ercilia; Louise S. Éthier, C. Jourdan-Ionescu
and
C. Lacharité. "L'intervention auprès des familles
négligentes." In
J. P. Pourtois, Blessure d'enfant, La maltraitance: théorie,
pratique et intervention, 173_212. Belgique:
De Boeck, 1995.
Pelton, Leroy. Chronic Neglect Symposium Proceedings, 1993, 8_10. National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Washington:
U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services, 1997.
Peterson, M. S.; A. J. Urquiza. The Role of Mental Health
Professionals in the Prevention and Treatment of Child Abuse and
Neglect. National Center on Child Abuse and Neglect. Washington:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1993.
Puckering C.; A. Pickles; D. Skuse; E. Heptinstall et al. "Mother_Child
Interaction and the Cognitive and Behavioural Development of Four-Year-Old
Children with Poor Growth." Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry 36(4) (1995): 573_595.
Ranjan, Roy. "Child Neglect and Pain." In Child Abuse
and Chronic Pain: A Curious Relationship?
58_73. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, 1998.
Reid, Grant; E. Sigurdson;
J. Christianson-Wood; A. Wright. "The Conceptualization of
Child Neglect." Winnipeg: University of Manitoba. Unpublished
paper, 1994.
Rose, Susan J.; W. Meezan.
"Defining Child Neglect: Evolution, Influences, and Issues." Social
Service Review 67(2) (1993):
279_293.
Rose, Susan J.; W. Meezan.
"Variations in Perceptions of Child Neglect." Child
Welfare 75(2) (1996): 139_160.
Rosenzweig, H. D.; S. J. Kaplan. "Child and adolescent neglect
and emotional maltreatment." In Family Violence: A Clinical
and Legal Guide, 37_71. Washington:
American Psychiatric Press, 1996.
Saunders, Edward J.; K. Nelson;
M. J. Landsman. "Racial Inequality and Child Neglect: Findings
in a Metropolitan Area." Child Welfare 72(4) (1993):
341_354.
Sedlak, Andrea J.; D. D. Broadhurst. Third National Incidence
Study of Child Abuse and Neglect.
Washington: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1996.
Sobsey, D.; W. Randall; R. K. Parrila. "Gender Differences
in Abused Children With and Without
Disabilities." Child Abuse and Neglect 21(8) (1997):
707_720.
Sullivan, P. M.; J. F. Knutson. "The Association Between Child
Maltreatment and Disabilities in a Hospital-Based Epidemiological
Study. Child Abuse and Neglect 22(4) (1998): 271_288.
Swift, Karen J. Manufacturing "Bad Mothers": A Critical Perspective on Child Neglect. Toronto: University of Toronto Press, 1995a.
Swift, Karen J. "An Outrage to Common Decency: Historical Perspectives on Child Neglect." Child Welfare 74(1) (1995b): 71_91.
Teti, D. M.; C. M. Gelfand;
D. S. Messinger; R. Isabella.
"Maternal Depression and the
Quality of Early Attachment: An Examination of Infants, Preschoolers,
and Their Mothers." Developmental Psychology 31 (1995):
364_376.
Toth, Sheree L. "A Developmental Psychopathology Perspective on Child Abuse and Neglect." Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 34(5) (1995): 541_565.
Tracy, E.; J. Whittaker; A. Pugh;
S. Kapp et al. "Support Networks of Primary Caregivers Receiving
Family Preservation Services: An Exploratory Study." Families
in Society 75 (1994): 481_489.
Tracy, E.; R. Green; M. Bremseth. "Meeting the Environmental Needs of Abused and Neglected Children: Implications from a Statewide Survey of Supportive Services." Social Work Research and Abstracts 29(2) (1993): 21_26.
Trickett, Penelope K.; McBride-Chang, Catherine. "The
Developmental Impact of Different Forms of Child Abuse and Neglect." Developmental Review 15 (1995): 311_337.
Trocmé, Nico. "Development and Preliminary Evaluation
of the
Ontario Child Neglect Index." Child Maltreatment 1(2)
(1996): 145_155.
Trocmé, Nico; D. McPhee;
K. K. Tam; T. Hay. Ontario Incidence Study of Reported Child
Abuse and Neglect. Toronto: Institute for the Prevention of
Child Abuse, 1994.
Trocmé, Nico; K. K. Tam;
D. McPhee. "Correlates of
Substantiation of Maltreatment in Child Welfare Investigations."
In
J. Hudson and B. Galaway (eds.), Child Welfare in Canada: Research
and Policy Implications, 20_40. Toronto: Thompson Educational
Publishing, 1995.
Weinberg, L. A. "Problems in
Educating Abused and Neglected Children with Disabilities." Child Abuse and Neglect 21(9) (1997): 889_905.
Wekerle, C.; D. A. Wolfe.
"Prevention of Child Physical Abuse and Neglect: Promising
New
Directions." Clinical Psychology Review 13(6) (1993):
501_540.
Weston, J.; M. Collaton. "A Legacy of Violence in Non-organic Failure to Thrive." Child Abuse and Neglect 17 (1993): 709_714.
Wiehe, Vernon R. Working with Child Abuse and Neglect: A Primer. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage, 1996.
Wolfe, David A. "Prevention of Child Neglect: Emerging Issues." Criminal Justice and Behaviour 20(1) (1993): 90_111.
Wolfe, David A.; C. Wekerle.
"Treatment Strategies for Child
Physical Abuse and Neglect: A Critical Progress Report." Clinical
Psychology Review 13 (1993):
473_500.
Zuravin, Susan; C. McMillen;
D. DePanfilis; C. Risley-Curtiss. "The Intergenerational Cycle
of Child Maltreatment: Continuity Versus Discontinuity." Journal
of Interpersonal Violence 11(3) (1996): 315_334.
To share this page just click on the social network icon of your choice.